全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1230篇 |
免费 | 104篇 |
国内免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
1359篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 4篇 |
2024年 | 13篇 |
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 30篇 |
2020年 | 52篇 |
2019年 | 49篇 |
2018年 | 78篇 |
2017年 | 39篇 |
2016年 | 35篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 43篇 |
2013年 | 78篇 |
2012年 | 42篇 |
2011年 | 41篇 |
2010年 | 34篇 |
2009年 | 45篇 |
2008年 | 39篇 |
2007年 | 44篇 |
2006年 | 32篇 |
2005年 | 45篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 73篇 |
2001年 | 100篇 |
2000年 | 78篇 |
1999年 | 54篇 |
1998年 | 44篇 |
1997年 | 25篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1359条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
This research advances a novel approach to promoting physical activity, based on the principle of functional matching in persuasion, and the self-concordance (SC) of people's motivations for physical activity. We propose that SC establishes a positive or negative orientation toward the challenge inherent in physical activity, and that the maximum yield of participation will be achieved by communications that appeal to each orientation. In two studies, we compared how messages emphasizing challenge versus available social support would influence recipients’ self-reported practices of physical activity and attitudes toward a physical activity setting. As hypothesized, these messages had differential effects for recipients whose pursuit of physical activity varied in SC, such that favorable outcomes were more reliably associated with challenge-oriented messages among respondents higher in SC versus support-oriented messages among respondents lower in SC. The findings suggest the merits of using self-regulatory, compared with object- or personality-based, constructs and measures to indicate psychological functions of ongoing health-related behaviors. 相似文献
192.
Erika Borkoles Adam R. Nicholls Kate Bell Ron Butterly Remco C. J. Polman 《Psychology & health》2013,28(4):427-441
The purpose of this study was to examine the lived experiences of people diagnosed with Multiple Sclerosis (MS). Seven active exercisers with MS participated in semi-structured interviews regarding their exercise experiences since diagnosis. Data were analysed using Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA; Smith?&?Osborn, 2003). Interpretive Phenomenological Analysis. The results and interpretations of narratives revealed a number of functional limitations due to the severity of MS symptoms, which were found to have a major effect on the ability of the participants to exercise. Furthermore, psychological problems and the heightened behavioural adjustments to the progressive disability led to re-appraisal of ability to exercise. Previous, relevant exercise experience made participants more determined to continue to be able to exercise after diagnosis. The wider exercise experience narratives were related to concerns about safety, dependability on others to overcome the challenges, and potential environmental hazards. The loss of spontaneous opportunities to exercise because of these actual and perceived barriers was key to this population. This research highlighted the need to rethink the health and social service arrangements in relation to exercise provision for individuals with MS. 相似文献
193.
Ryan M. Nefdt 《Inquiry (Oslo, Norway)》2013,56(8):877-911
Words form a fundamental basis for our understanding of linguistic practice. However, the precise ontology of words has eluded many philosophers and linguists. A persistent difficulty for most accounts of words is the type-token distinction [Bromberger, S. 1989. “Types and Tokens in Linguistics.” In Reflections on Chomsky, edited by A. George, 58–90. Basil Blackwell; Kaplan, D. 1990. “Words.” Aristotelian Society Supplementary Volume LXIV: 93–119]. In this paper, I present a novel account of words which differs from the atomistic and platonistic conceptions of previous accounts which I argue fall prey to this problem. Specifically, I proffer a structuralist account of linguistic items, along the lines of structuralism in the philosophy of mathematics [Shapiro, S. 1997. Philosophy of Mathematics: Structure and Ontology. Oxford University Press], in which words are defined in part as positions in larger linguistic structures. I then follow Szabò [1999. “Expressions and Their Representations.” The Philosophical Quarterly 49 (195): 145–163] and Parsons [1990. “The Structuralist View of Mathematical Objects.” Synthese 84: 303–346] in further defining words as quasi-concrete objects according to a representation relation. This view aims for general correspondence with contemporary generative linguistic approaches to the study of language. 相似文献
194.
195.
The experiments described in the lead articles by Kowalski and Zimiles and by O'Hanlon and Roberson examine factors that lead to color term acquisition. These experiments touch on the debate regarding the relative contributions of language and concepts in word learning. In this reflection, we examine how conclusions concerning the debate depend deeply on the particular task presented to children, and we propose an alternative approach to studying color term acquisition. 相似文献
196.
M. Massazza G. Riontino A. Dupasquier R. Ferragut A. Somoza P. Folegati 《Philosophical Magazine Letters》2013,93(9):495-502
Secondary ageing, that is microstructural evolution occurring at room temperature after short heating at temperatures above the metastable phase boundary of Guinier-Preston zones, has been studied for an Al-Cu-Mg alloy with a high Cu-to-Mg ratio. Combined data from positron annihilation spectroscopy, Vickers microhardness measurements and differential scanning calorimetry show that, on secondary ageing after 5 or 7min at 190°C, firstly, hardening takes place at a rate nine to 16 times slower than natural age hardening; secondly, vacancies slowly released by Cu-rich aggregates formed during the heat treatment at 190°C promote further formation of solute aggregates, with a time-dependent chemical composition; thirdly, the thermal stability of the structures formed during secondary ageing increases with increasing dwell time at room temperature; and, fourthly, solute aggregates formed at 190°C undergo a structural reorganization and possibly a change in the composition, leading to species with a different thermal stability. The slow release of vacancies from Cu-rich aggregates is proposed as one of the limiting factors of the hardening rate. 相似文献
197.
《The journal of positive psychology》2013,8(3):216-223
Analyses examined both positive emotion (PE) at baseline and change in PE from baseline to follow up as predictors of change in functional status (FS). Initial models examined baseline PE, and change in PE, as predictors of change in FS adjusted for baseline PE, negative emotion (NE), FS, and age and gender. Final models included additional adjustments for frequency of social contact, marital status, body mass index, smoking status, drinking behavior, and comorbidity at baseline. Baseline PE was a predictor of change in FS, adjusted for NE, age, and gender (p?=?0.033). Additional covariates reduced the effect of baseline PE as a predictor of change in FS (p?=?0.115). Change in PE was a significant predictor of change in FS – increases in ratings of PE over time were associated with less decline in FS (p?=?0.001). The association for change in PE remained significant in the fully adjusted model (p?=?0.004). Results replicate our prior findings. 相似文献
198.
《The journal of positive psychology》2013,8(4):337-345
The idea that eudaimonic well-being (EWB) should be distinguished from the more widespread notion of hedonic well-being (HWB), has stirred up disagreement among happiness researchers. Siding with EWB researchers, this study provides theoretical and empirical arguments supporting the distinctiveness and usefulness of a EWB dimension. A path model with data from Norwegian university students (N?=?184) showed that, when requested to draw both a happy face and a sad face, indicators of HWB were associated with a preference for working with the happy rather than the sad face. Indicators of EWB were uncorrelated with this hedonic bias. Eudaimonic feeling states were associated with the level of creativity involved in the drawings, while hedonic feelings were not. Finally, participants who draw bigger faces also felt more pleasure during the act of drawing. Eudaimonic feelings were unrelated to the size of the drawing. 相似文献
199.
Robert J. McDonaldNancy S. Hong Colleen RayMartin R. Ralph 《Learning and motivation》2002,33(2):230-252
Various demonstrations of “time stamp” effects in the animal learning literature have reinforced the idea that circadian information is encoded as part of a combined internal/external representation of context and that this contextual information is utilized for complex retrieval processes supporting memory. The goal of the present series of experiments is to assess this idea by manipulating training/testing circadian times on a battery of learning and memory tasks commonly used in the rodent. The data obtained from five experiments using four different learning and memory paradigms provide no evidence for “time stamp” effects on place memory, context memory (aversive or appetitive), or S-R habit learning. 相似文献
200.
本研究基于广州市基础教育质量监测项目,以2380名初中生及其家长为被试,构建一个有调节的中介模型,考察了家校合作对青少年学业成绩的影响机制。研究发现:(1)家校合作对青少年学业成绩具有显著的正向预测作用;(2)在家校合作对青少年学业成绩的影响中,亲子沟通发挥了部分中介作用;(3)独生状况调节了家校合作→亲子沟通→学业成绩的后半段,即相对于非独生子女而言,独生子女亲子沟通对学业成绩的促进作用更为显著。 相似文献