全文获取类型
收费全文 | 481篇 |
免费 | 28篇 |
专业分类
509篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 14篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 43篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 8篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 23篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 26篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 16篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有509条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
John S. Watson Louise A. Hayes Peter Vietze 《Journal of applied developmental psychology》1982,3(3):191-203
An 8-month-old infant with a developmental quotient of months was given response-contingent stimulation using a pressure-sensitive pillow which turned an overhead mobile. The subject learned to control the mobile by kicking the pillow, and concurrently began smiling at both the mobile and her mother for the first time. After mastering three contingencies on arm, head, and leg movement, she displayed what appeared to be a Piagetian coordinated secondary circular reaction, in which one response provided 4 seconds of access to another contingency. Although the subject remains severely retarded, the results suggest that some forms of developmental delay may be treated at least in part as a failure to develop contingency awareness. 相似文献
22.
Thirty-two infants aged 14 and 20 weeks were presented with a live face in each of eight conditions, which consisted of all combinations of (a) a 0° or 90° orientation; (b) familiar face (the infant's mother) or distinctively unfamiliar face; and (c) talking or silent context. The previous findings that younger infants smile longer at 0° than at 90° faces and that this differential responsiveness to orientation wanes with increasing age were replicated; the hypothesis that older infants would smile longest at their mothers' talking faces in the 0° orientation was confirmed. In addition, infants of both ages smiled more at their mothers than at the stranger, although this effect interacted with orientation and sex of the infant. 相似文献
23.
24.
On the basis of cybernetic models of speech it was hypothesized that “stuttering” induced by delayed auditory feedback could be significantly reduced if the Ss were provided with an alternate input to which they could attend. The results of the experiment supported the hypothesis. The possible implications for the treatment stuttering are discussed. 相似文献
25.
Three patients are described who displayed syntactic writing errors in combination with a motor speech disturbance and impaired motor limb function. Two of the patients had bulbar amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). Agraphia appeared when verbal communication was no longer possible. Autopsy in one patient disclosed only lesions consistent with ALS. The third patient had palilalia and chorea and, although not aphasic, his written language showed persistent syntactic errors. We hypothesize that the agraphia in these patients occurred because of the combination of disordered feedback from the motor speech apparatus and limbs. 相似文献
26.
In three studies the formulation was tested that characteristics of the observer have an effect on imitation only in situations which provide little information concerning appropriate or expected behaviors. In Experiment I, female college students of high, medium, and low need for social approval underwent either a high or low arousal manipulation. Subjects observed a videotaped model who engaged in a size judgment task. For half the subjects the model received verbal reward (high information); for the other half the model received no feedback (low information). Subsequently, the model and subject responded alternately in the task during which no feedback was given. Neither need for social approval nor manipulated arousal was found to be systematically related to imitation in either information condition. However, the hypothesized relationships were found when subjects' arousal or anxiety levels were determined by self ratings. These results were replicated in a second, similar experiment. In a third experiment, which involved an improved arousal manipulation, the hypothesized relationships were confirmed. 相似文献
27.
28.
John M Dillard 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1976,9(3):311-320
The relationship between career maturity and self-concepts of 252 sixth-grade black males was investigated. Student samples of suburban and urban middle- and urban lower-class were drawn from 42 different school settings located in Monroe, Erie, and Westchester Counties of New York State. The Attitude Scale of the Career Maturity Inventory and the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory were used to assess the association of career maturity and self-concepts. Results indicated relatively weak-positive relationships between career maturity and self-concepts. Significant career maturity differences were found. However, self-concepts of the three groups were not statistically significantly different. Of the set of independent variables predicting and estimating variance accounted for (socioeconomic status, family intactness, place of residence, and reading), socioeconomic status was found to have the strongest predictive value on career maturity. It was suggested that researchers examine elementary school black students' career development in view of the group with whom they identify most. 相似文献
29.
Four hamsters received brief electric shocks contingent upon running in a wheel. The fixed interval between shocks was varied systematically from 0 to 60 sec. Time spent running was directly related to the length of the fixed interval, while running speed remained roughly constant. Running depended on the fixed interval between shocks in the same way as bar-pressing maintained by food reinforcement, despite the fact that running was not established with nor maintained by explicit reinforcement. 相似文献
30.