首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   225篇
  免费   31篇
  国内免费   27篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   10篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   17篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   9篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   22篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   8篇
  2013年   30篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   4篇
  2010年   1篇
  2009年   11篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   14篇
  2006年   16篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   10篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   1篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   2篇
  1991年   2篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
排序方式: 共有283条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
The purpose of this study was to replicate and extend research on task interspersal. The authors investigated whether changes in on-task behavior of two middle school students were functionally related to changes in the relative percentages of easy and difficult items on math worksheets. They found that the participants remained on task longer while completing worksheets with 33 and 67% interspersed easy problems than while completing worksheets without the interspersed easy problems. Participants’ accuracy in answering the target problems was not affected, however, by the interspersal procedure. The authors concluded that interspersing easy items on independent math seatwork assignments can increase on-task behavior.  相似文献   
102.
Jessi L. Smith 《Sex roles》2006,54(3-4):287-296
This project was designed to examine whether and how achievement goals contribute to the effect of gender stereotypes on women's low expectancies for success on math tasks. Study 1 confirmed predictions from the Stereotyped Task Engagement Process (STEP) model (Smith, 2004) by demonstrating that, compared to a counter-stereotype situation, women reminded of the gender-stereotype endorsed performance-avoidance goals. Study 2 tested whether performance-goal adoption differed between men and women. Results showed that, compared to men, women in a stereotype salient math situation were more likely to endorse performance-avoidance achievement goals. This performance-avoidance goal adoption in turn, accounted for the negative relationship between participants' gender and performance expectations for a standardized math test.  相似文献   
103.
算术应用题的分类结果与数学成绩关系研究   总被引:3,自引:1,他引:2  
阴国恩  冯虹 《心理科学》2006,29(4):778-781
以算术应用题为材料,探讨了不同年级、不同数学成绩学生对算术应用题的分类结果及其与数学成绩的关系。结果表明:不同年级学生对算术应用题分类结果差异显著;数学成绩优生与数学成绩差生对算术应用题分类结果存在差异。  相似文献   
104.
How does improving children's ability to label set sizes without counting affect the development of understanding of the cardinality principle? It may accelerate development by facilitating subsequent alignment and comparison of the cardinal label for a given set and the last word counted when counting that set (Mix et al., 2012). Alternatively, it may delay development by decreasing the need for a comprehensive abstract principle to understand and label exact numerosities (Piantadosi et al., 2012). In this study, preschoolers (N = 106, Mage = 4;8) were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: (a) count‐and‐label, wherein children spent 6 weeks both counting and labeling sets arranged in canonical patterns like pips on a die; (b) label‐first,wherein children spent the first 3 weeks learning to label the set sizes without counting before spending 3 weeks identical to the count‐and‐label condition; (c) print referencing control. Both counting conditions improved understanding of cardinality through increases in children's ability to label set sizes without counting. In addition to this indirect effect, there was a direct effect of the count‐and‐label condition on progress toward understanding of cardinality. Results highlight the roles of set labeling and equifinality in the development of children's understanding of number concepts.  相似文献   
105.
使用Crack the Code任务考察单纯型与混合型数学困难小学儿童的执行水平,结果发现:单纯型数学困难儿童在对认知资源要求较高的任务上的执行水平显著低于数学优良生,但在认知要求较低的任务上二者之间没有显著差异;混合型数学困难儿童执行水平最低,在两个难度水平执行任务上的表现均显著差于数学优良生、单纯型数学困难儿童.小学儿童执行水平年级差异似乎主要表现在认知负荷较高的任务上,1年级学生与3、5年级学生的执行水平存在显著差异.  相似文献   
106.
A within-subjects design was used to compare explicit timing and interspersal with college students. Students were given 3 minutes to complete problems on the explicit timing assignment (25 problems, 3 digits –3 digits) and the interspersal assignment (25 similar problems and 10 problems, 1 digit –1 digit). Results indicated that: (a) students completed more total problems during interspersal for both trials, (b) students completed more target problems during explicit timing for the second trial, and (c) students only preferred interspersal for the first trial. The data from trial one fit the discrete task completion hypothesis and matching law, yet the data from trial two do not match as closely (Skinner, 2002). Discussion focuses on continued need for more research on academic interventions, comparing academic interventions, the discrete task completion hypothesis, and the matching law.  相似文献   
107.
Within the context of an experimental analysis, the current study examined the effects of student- and empirically-selected interventions on the mathematics computational fluency of three elementary-aged students. For all of the participants, the empirically-selected intervention resulted in higher levels of computational fluency then the student-selected intervention. These results suggest that empirically-selected interventions may enhance the mathematics computational fluency of students experiencing mathematics problems. However, individual responsiveness to empirically-selected interventions suggests the importance of conducting brief experimental analyses to determine the most efficacious intervention. The implication of these results for intervention selection and incorporating choice-making opportunities into academic interventions are discussed.  相似文献   
108.
Our aim is to point out some differences between verbal and visual arguments, promoting the rhetorical perspective of argumentation beyond the relevance of logic and pragmatics. In our view, if it is to be rational and successful, film as (visual) argumentation must be addressed to spectators who hold informed beliefs about the theme watched on the screen and the medium’s constraints and conventions. In our reflections to follow, we apply rhetorical analysis to film as a symbolic, human, and communicative act that may sometimes be understood as a visually laid out argument. As a mixture of visual, auditory, and verbal stimuli, film demands active and complex interpretation and (re)construction. Our suggestion is to focus on five different but interrelated elements. The reconstruction and evaluation of the visual argument will be based on those elements, and the whole process will be one of visual argumentation.
Jesús Alcolea-BanegasEmail:
  相似文献   
109.
In the Netherlands, national assessments at the end of primary school (Grade 6) show a decline of achievement on problems of complex or written arithmetic over the last two decades. The present study aims at contributing to an explanation of the large achievement decrease on complex division, by investigating the strategies students used in solving the division problems in the two most recent assessments carried out in 1997 and in 2004. The students’ strategies were classified into four categories. A data set resulted with two types of repeated observations within students: the nominal strategies and the dichotomous achievement scores (correct/incorrect) on the items administered.It is argued that latent variable modeling methodology is appropriate to analyze these data. First, latent class analyses with year of assessment as a covariate were carried out on the multivariate nominal strategy variables. Results showed a shift from application of the traditional long division algorithm in 1997, to the less accurate strategy of stating an answer without writing down any notes or calculations in 2004, especially for boys. Second, explanatory IRT analyses showed that the three main strategies were significantly less accurate in 2004 than they were in 1997.  相似文献   
110.
Using an alternating treatments design, we investigated the effects of self-recording and goal-setting on the on-task behavior and the mathematics productivity of five low-achieving elementary school students in a group of eleven students in Korea. We also examined which intervention students preferred after they experienced self-recording and goal-setting. Visual analyses of the data showed (a) increases in students' on-task and mathematics productivity levels contingent upon the implementation of both self-recording and goal-setting, (b) no differential effects of the two interventions, and (c) students' general preference of goal-setting to self recording. We interpreted these results as indicating that both self-recording and goal-setting are effective self-management strategies, but practitioners must consider the features of the interventions before selecting one from them, and future researchers must examine the effect of combining the two procedures.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号