全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2910篇 |
免费 | 484篇 |
国内免费 | 195篇 |
专业分类
3589篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 16篇 |
2024年 | 53篇 |
2023年 | 92篇 |
2022年 | 105篇 |
2021年 | 139篇 |
2020年 | 178篇 |
2019年 | 165篇 |
2018年 | 140篇 |
2017年 | 156篇 |
2016年 | 160篇 |
2015年 | 152篇 |
2014年 | 155篇 |
2013年 | 375篇 |
2012年 | 123篇 |
2011年 | 132篇 |
2010年 | 114篇 |
2009年 | 130篇 |
2008年 | 140篇 |
2007年 | 121篇 |
2006年 | 134篇 |
2005年 | 117篇 |
2004年 | 108篇 |
2003年 | 86篇 |
2002年 | 71篇 |
2001年 | 54篇 |
2000年 | 50篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 25篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有3589条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
942.
作为学习时间分配研究热点的基于议程调节模型认为,学习者是在学习目标的指导下,建构并执行议程来进行学习时间分配。议程即学习计划,是学习时间分配的驱动力。该模型强调学习者在自我调节学习过程中有意识的进行着学习效益最大化的权衡决策来实现学习目标,包容了以往学习时间分配的经典理论。大量的新近研究支持了基于议程调节模型,并深入地探讨了议程驱动与习惯性反应之间的关系。 未来的研究应进一步考察学习时间分配权衡过程中的意识和无意识双加工机制,探讨个体变量对学习时间分配议程建构的权衡机制的影响,并在更加生态化的情境中考察学习时间分配议程建构的权衡机制。 相似文献
943.
本研究以4岁~5岁儿童认知能力测验为例,在IRT框架下探讨了如何进行追踪数据的测量不变性分析。分析模型采用项目间多维项目反应理论模型(between-item MIRT model)和项目内(within-item MIRT model)多维two-tier model,被试为来自全国的882名48个月的儿童,工具为自编4岁~5岁儿童认知能力测验。经测验水平 分析和项目水平分析,结果表明:(1)本文对追踪数据的测量不变性分析方法合理有效; (2)该测验在两个时间点上满足部分测量不变性要求,测验的潜在结构稳定; (3)“方位题”的区分度和难度参数都发生变化,另有4题难度参数出现浮动; (4)儿童在4岁~5岁期间认知能力总体呈快速发展趋势,能力增长显著。 相似文献
944.
Ten individuals with dizziness participated in a longitudinal study on the relationship between stress and dizziness. The participants rated dizziness and stress on visual analogue scales twice daily for a period of 28 days (in all 56 data points). The ratings of stress included physical stress, mental stress, emotional stress, and the presence of stressful events. The data was analysed by means of time-series analysis (ARIMA), and the temporal associations investigated by lagged correlations. Results showed concurrent associations between dizziness and mental and emotional stress. However, individual differences were observed indicating complex and diverse patterns of association between different forms of stress and dizziness. 相似文献
945.
Roch SG Lane JA Samuelson CD Allison ST Dent JL 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2000,83(2):724-212
Two studies were conducted to test a two-stage model of the psychological mechanisms underlying the overconsumption of scarce resources in small groups. The model proposes that members of groups sharing resources first anchor their consumption choices on an “equal-division” heuristic and then, given sufficient cognitive capacity, adjust their choices in a self-serving direction. The results from both studies support the model. The first study found that when faced with a common resource pool almost all group members expressed thoughts regarding equality; however, individuals with sufficient cognitive capacity requested more from the pool and expressed more task-relevant thoughts than individuals lacking the necessary cognitive resources. The second study provided additional support for the two-stage model by demonstrating that group members' cognitions occur in the order predicted by the model and by demonstrating that an individual difference, social value orientation, affects thought processes only when individuals are not experiencing high cognitive loads. Implications are discussed. 相似文献
946.
人类基因组计划与医学模式 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
谢蜀生 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2000,21(9):20-21
人类基因组计划将为医学科学的发展提供广泛的可能性,并结生物医学带来革命性的变化,但是人类的社会性,疾病产生的非生物因素以及社会及伦理的限制都决定了任何生物医学上的成就,包括人类基因组计划都不能改变生物社会心理医学模式的深刻内涵。 相似文献
947.
948.
The tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance: A replication with adolescent girls 总被引:4,自引:1,他引:3
The tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance is a recent theoretical approach that includes a test of direct (peer, parental, and media factors) and mediational links (internalization of societal appearance standards, appearance comparison processes) as factors potentially leading to body dissatisfaction and eating disturbance. The theory was evaluated in the current study in a sample of 391 adolescent females. A structural equation model that evaluated the tripartite model replicated previous findings reported by Keery, van den Berg and Thompson (2004) [Keery, H., van den Berg, & Thompson, J. K. (2004). A test of the tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance in adolescent girls. Body Image: An International Journal of Research, 1, 237–251.]. Additionally, a second model extended these findings, suggesting that peer and media influences are more important than parental influences. The results are discussed in light of the need for prevention programs to incorporate formative influences and mediational processes in the construction of intervention strategies. 相似文献
949.
Geometric alterations to the boundaries of a virtual environment were used to investigate the representations underlying human spatial memory. Subjects encountered a cue object in a simple rectangular enclosure, with distant landmarks for orientation. After a brief delay, during which they were removed from the arena, subjects were returned to it at a new location and orientation and asked to mark the place where the cue had been. On some trials the geometry (size, aspect ratio) of the arena was varied between presentation and testing. Responses tended to lie somewhere between a location that maintained fixed distances from nearby walls and a location that maintained fixed ratios of the distances between opposing walls. The former were more common after expansions and for cued locations nearer to the edge while the latter were more common after contractions and for locations nearer to the center. The spatial distributions of responses predicted by various simple geometric models were compared to the data. The best fitting model was one derived from the response properties of 'place cells' in the rat hippocampus, which matches the 'proximities' 1/(d+c) of the cue to the four walls of the arena, where d is the distance to a wall and c is a global constant. Subjects also tended to adopt the same orientation at presentation and testing, although this was not due to using a view matching strategy, which could be ruled out in 50% of responses. Disoriented responses were most often seen where the cued location was near the center of the arena or where the long axis of a rectangular arena was changed between presentation and testing, suggesting that the geometry of the arena acts as a weak cue to orientation. Overall, the results suggest a process of visual landmark matching to determine orientation, combined with an abstract representation of the proximity of the cued location to the walls of the arena consistent with the neural representation of location in the hippocampus. 相似文献
950.
Glickman, Gray, and Morales (this issue) propose a statistical model for measuring the unobserved latency of stimulus-controlled
processes. The model accounts for both speed and accuracy and does so by assuming participants set an internal deadline. If
a stimulus-controlled response is not produced by the deadline, the participant then guesses. The applicability the model
is discussed in this comment. The deadline model yields specific predictions for the case in which stimulus difficulty is
manipulated in a within-block manner. In this case, it is reasonable to assume that stimulus difficulty does not affect the
deadline. It is shown that in common perceptual and cognitive domains, extant data do not fully meet these predictions. Hence,
practitioners need be aware of the possibility and consequences of model misspecification.
This research is supported by NSF grant SES - 0095919 to J. Rouder, D. Sun, and P. Speckman. 相似文献