全文获取类型
收费全文 | 434篇 |
免费 | 23篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
459篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 16篇 |
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 26篇 |
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 25篇 |
2016年 | 27篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 82篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 9篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 8篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有459条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
181.
Ulrich Orth Matthias Berking Nadine Walker Laurenz L. Meier Hansjörg Znoj 《Journal of research in personality》2008
Forgiveness is often assumed to be adaptive for psychological adjustment following interpersonal transgressions. Three hundred and forty seven individuals who had experienced a recent interpersonal transgression were surveyed on four occasions over the course of six weeks. Forgiveness was assessed with scales measuring interpersonal avoidance and revenge motivation and psychological adjustment was assessed with scales measuring depression and rumination. Latent growth curve analyses showed that intraindividual changes in forgiveness were positively correlated with changes in adjustment. Latent difference score analyses indicated that adjustment predicted subsequent change in forgiveness, but that forgiveness did not predict subsequent change in adjustment. The results suggest that adjustment facilitates forgiveness, but not that forgiveness facilitates adjustment. 相似文献
182.
A mathematical model is described based on the first order system transfer function in the form Y=B3∗exp(−B2∗(X−1))+B4∗(1−exp(−B2∗(X−1))), where X is the learning session number; Y is the quantity of errors, B2 is the learning rate, B3 is resistance to learning and B4 is ability to learn. The model is tested in a light-dark discrimination learning task in a 3-arm radial maze using Wistar and albino rats. The model provided good fits of experimental data under acquisition and reacquisition, and was able to detect strain differences among Wistar and albino rats. The model was compared to Rescorla-Wagner, and was found to be mutually complementary. Comparisons with Tulving’s logarithmic function and Valentine’s hyperbola and the arc cotangent functions are also provided. Our model is valid for fitting averaged group data, if averaging is applied to a subgroup of subjects possessing individual learning curves of an exponential shape. 相似文献
183.
This study investigated high school students’ process of choosing a study in higher education, using a longitudinal design. A sample of 535 adolescents participated at the beginning, middle, and end of Grade 12. Latent curve modeling revealed evidence for a mean developmental increase in the career decisional tasks of orientation, exploration, decisional status, and commitment. In general, the growth trajectories were in line with theoretical models in which orientation and broad exploration are important at the beginning of the career decision-making process whereas in-depth exploration and decisional status are considered important later on. Furthermore, the results showed substantial variability between adolescents in their initial levels of career decisional tasks and in the rate of change. Finally, growth in orientation related to growth in environmental and self-exploration, and growth in environmental exploration related to progress in commitment and decisional status. 相似文献
184.
Judi Mesman Reinoud Stoel Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg Marinus H. van IJzendoorn Femmie Juffer Hans M. Koot Lenneke R.A. Alink 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2009,37(5):625-636
Using an accelerated longitudinal design, the development of externalizing problems from age 2 to 5 years was investigated
in relation to maternal psychopathology, maternal parenting, gender, child temperament, and the presence of siblings. The
sample consisted of 150 children selected at age 2–3 years for having high levels of externalizing problems. Parenting was
measured using observational methods, and maternal reports were used for the other variables. Overall, mean levels of externalizing
problems decreased over time, and higher initial levels (intercept) were related to a stronger decrease (negative slope) in
externalizing problems. Results showed that higher levels of maternal psychopathology were related to less decrease in early
childhood externalizing problems. Parental sensitive behavior predicted a stronger decrease in externalizing problems, but
only for children with difficult temperaments. A stronger decrease of externalizing problems in children with older siblings
also pertained only to children with difficult temperaments. Thus, temperamentally difficult children appear to be more susceptible
to environmental influences on the development of externalizing behaviors. Our results indicate that the role of siblings
in early childhood externalizing problems deserves more research attention, and that intervention efforts need to take into
account temperamental differences in children’s susceptibility to environmental influences. 相似文献
185.
Mathiesen KS Sanson A Stoolmiller M Karevold E 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2009,37(2):209-222
Using growth curve modeling, trajectories of undercontrolled (oppositional, irritable, inattentive and overactive behaviors)
and internalizing (worried, sad and fearful) problems from 18 months to 4.5 years were studied in a population based sample
of 921 Norwegian children. At the population level, undercontrolled problems decreased and internalizing problems increased
with increasing age. Child temperament and family factors present at 18 months predicted 43% of the stability and 20% of the
linear changes in undercontrolled problems and 30% of the stability and 7% of the linear changes in internalizing problems.
Persisting effects of child and family factors from 18 month were found on the two problem dimensions over and above the impact
of changes in the same risk factors. Lower initial level of partner support and higher initial level of child emotionality
predicted higher slope of undercontrolled problems and higher initial level of family stress predicted higher slope for internalizing
problems. As expected, time to time change (t1 to t2 and t2 to t3) in risk factors predicted time to time change in problem scores as well. The study shows the significance of early emerging
internalizing and undercontrolled problems, the need to consider their pathways separately from very young ages, lasting effects
of early experiences, and the importance of a dynamic approach to the analysis of risk. 相似文献
186.
Novice acquisition of skilled recall of chess positions was studied in an experiment in which two novices studied a series of five hundred chess positions during a period of several months. They spent fifteen minutes to half an hour a day teaching themselves these positions. As a result their skill in recalling chess positions rose from sixteen percent to somewhere between forty to fifty percent. The learning curve proved to have a shape which indicates that in the beginning learning is very fast but after some 100-150 studied positions the speed of learning decreases substantially. A computer simulation was used to model the results and analyse alternative explanations. Two alternative ways of thinking were tested. In the first, chunk construction was assumed to be based on the neighbourhood of associated pieces. The second model assumed a frequency-based correlative association process. Although the learning curves of the two models are very similar in shape to those of the subjects, the frequency-based associative model gave a better explanation for the data. This is why it is natural to suggest that common co-occurrence in addition to easily recognizable chess-specific characteristics, like colour and type of pieces, guide associative processes during chess players' learning of chess-specific chunks. 相似文献
187.
Statistical application of signal detection theory has been used to study the clinical utility of early treatment response in a range of treatments and psychiatric disorders. The current study sought to examine the predictive value of weekly within-treatment drinking using receiver operator curves (ROCs) and zero-inflated Poisson (ZIP) regression in 102 women with alcohol use disorders (AUDs) randomized to either alcohol behavioral individual treatment (ABIT; n = 52) or alcohol behavioral couples treatment (ABCT; n = 50). ROC analyses indicated that failure to achieve or sustain abstinence by the end-of-treatment and one-year follow-up was predicted with reasonable accuracy by week 4 percent days abstinent (PDA) in ABIT. ZIP models yielded similar results with evidence for within-treatment PDA with week 6 PDA predicting both the abstinence as well as percent days drinking at the end-of-treatment and one-year follow-up. Within-treatment PDA was a significantly better predictor of outcomes for ABIT than ABCT, despite a better overall treatment response for ABCT. Implications for stepped care models of alcohol treatment are discussed and recommendations for future research made. When Should Clinicians Switch Treatments: An Application of Signal Detection Theory to Two Treatments for Women with Alcohol Use Disorders. 相似文献
188.
This paper investigates the psychometric properties of the values in action (VIA) character strengths (Peterson and Seligman, 2004). A sample of 904 South African undergraduate students (female=77%, male=23%, black=70%, mean age=21.07 years, SD age=2.73 years) was assessed using a 380-item questionnaire that included the items from the international personality item pool (IPIP) values in action (VIA) measure of 24 character strengths as well as additional items based on the underlying theory of the particular constructs. Responses were analysed with the Rasch rating scale model. Reliability coefficients were computed for the retained scale items. The majority (21) of the scales demonstrated satisfactory Rasch model fit and good reliability of scores. The finding that a large proportion of strengths exhibited differential item functioning for at least one of (1) gender, (2) ethnicity and (3) home language group, challenges the assumption that character strengths are necessarily accultural, indicating qualitative distinctions in construct conceptualisations and measurement as a function of emic factors. 相似文献
189.
以22项实证研究(25个独立样本,79665个被试)为对象,综合使用元分析和结构方程模型,以家庭投资模型和家庭压力模型为理论基础,探讨了家庭社会经济地位对学业成就的影响及内在机制。结果发现:(1)家庭社会经济地位能显著地正向预测学业成就(r=0.32);(2)家庭社会经济地位测量方式的调节效应显著;(3)父母卷入在家庭社会经济地位与学业成就间起着部分中介作用;(4)父母卷入的中介效应只存在于小学及以下群体,在中学群体中介效应不显著。结论,家庭社会经济地位与学业成就之间存在显著正相关,在小学和幼儿园阶段时父母卷入在家庭社会经济地位和学业成就间起着部分中介作用。 相似文献
190.
We investigated the effects of grade retention in first grade on the growth of the Woodcock-Johnson broad mathematics and reading scores over three years using linear growth curve modeling on an academically at-risk sample. A large sample (n = 784) of first grade children who were at risk for retention was initially identified based on low literacy scores. Scores representing propensity for retention were constructed based on 72 variables collected in comprehensive baseline testing in first grade. We closely matched 97 pairs of retained and promoted children based on their propensity scores using optimal matching procedures. This procedure adjusted for baseline differences between the retained and promoted children. We found that grade retention decreased the growth rate of mathematical skills but had no significant effect on reading skills. In addition, several potential moderators of the effect of retention on growth of mathematical and reading skills were identified including limited English language proficiency and children's conduct problems. 相似文献