全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4875篇 |
免费 | 597篇 |
国内免费 | 516篇 |
专业分类
5988篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 14篇 |
2023年 | 114篇 |
2022年 | 111篇 |
2021年 | 152篇 |
2020年 | 271篇 |
2019年 | 263篇 |
2018年 | 204篇 |
2017年 | 288篇 |
2016年 | 295篇 |
2015年 | 186篇 |
2014年 | 240篇 |
2013年 | 711篇 |
2012年 | 162篇 |
2011年 | 190篇 |
2010年 | 159篇 |
2009年 | 218篇 |
2008年 | 275篇 |
2007年 | 232篇 |
2006年 | 218篇 |
2005年 | 188篇 |
2004年 | 174篇 |
2003年 | 146篇 |
2002年 | 135篇 |
2001年 | 103篇 |
2000年 | 101篇 |
1999年 | 89篇 |
1998年 | 61篇 |
1997年 | 50篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 45篇 |
1994年 | 49篇 |
1993年 | 31篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 32篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 33篇 |
1984年 | 34篇 |
1983年 | 22篇 |
1982年 | 33篇 |
1981年 | 33篇 |
1980年 | 35篇 |
1979年 | 31篇 |
1978年 | 29篇 |
1977年 | 31篇 |
1976年 | 28篇 |
1975年 | 21篇 |
1974年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有5988条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
William Irons 《Zygon》1991,26(1):49-89
Abstract. This paper presents and criticizes. Alexander's evolutionary theory of morality (1987). Earlier research, on which Alexander's theory is based, is also reviewed. The propensity to create moral systems evolved because it allowed ancestral humans to limit conflict within cooperating groups and thus form larger groups, which were advantageous because of intense between-group competition. Alexander sees moral codes as contractual, and the primary criticism of his theory is that moral codes are not completely contractual but also coercive. Ways of evaluating Alexander's theory as well as modified versions of it are discussed. 相似文献
62.
The second offspring of General Process Learning Theory: Overt behavior as the ambassador of the mind 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Malone JC 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1982,38(2):205-209
Wasserman suggested in a recent book review that the study of intervening cognitive processes represents a current focus of interest in animal learning and that this has led to a revitalization of comparative psychology. An examination of the volume reviewed suggests that he may have overstated the case. Most of the authors to whom he refers expressed dissatisfaction with traditional stimulus-response associationism but few argued for the extreme (information processing) sort of cognitive approach described by Wasserman. 相似文献
63.
This study investigated recognition memory of self-photographs which had been previously rated by subjects for their degree of reference to an imaginal prototype of “real self.” The number of false alarms to NEW photographs was found to increase with degree of self-reference to the imaginal prototype with the exception of photographs rated highest in self-reference. Females were reliably superior to males in self-recognition. Males and females also differed in nonverbal behaviors reflecting differences in their image of real self. Results were interpreted as supporting an hypothesis that the self functions as a cognitive prototype which has both imaginal and verbal characteristics. 相似文献
64.
Robert Pryor 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1982,20(1):40-52
There have been comparatively few attempts on either the conceptual or empirical level to integrate values, preferences, needs, work ethics, and orientations to work. A rationale outlining the way these concepts can be related to each other, is presented. It is maintained that these concepts are categories of relations between humans and work. These categories are qualitatively similar and differ principally in the level of generality of the characteristics of work considered. Complete-link cluster analysis is suggested as a method of analysis compatible with the proffered rationale. Two studies are presented with data gathered during the development of two versions of Work Aspect Preference Scale (WAPS). The samples for the two studies both comprised senior high school students, controlled for sex and age, who were administered the WAPS. Study 1 had a sample of 532 while Study 2 had a sample size of 482. Both sets of data were cluster analyzed. Results indicated a very substantial amount of agreement between the two samples, particularly at the more general levels of clustering. The bipartite and tripartite groupings of vocational needs suggested by L. H. Lofquist and R. V. Dawis (Journal of Vocational Behavior, 1978, 12, 12–19) were confirmed as were the findings of J. Mortimer (Sociology of Work and Occupations, 1975, 2, 29–53). Other clusters were also identified with findings from previous research. It is concluded that the investigation of preferences at different levels of generality and the interrelation between these levels shows great promise for both the researcher and the counselor and therefore merits more research effort. 相似文献
65.
Stimulus discriminability in free-operant and discrete-trial detection procedures. 总被引:4,自引:4,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
D McCarthy M Davison P E Jenkins 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1982,37(2):199-215
Six pigeons were trained to discriminate different light intensities in four experimental procedures. Experiment 1 compared stimulus discriminability in a yes-no signal-detection task with discriminability measures obtained from two free-operant procedures. Discriminability estimates were significantly lower in the detection procedure. Experiment 2 showed this lowered discriminability to be a function of the delay between stimulus presentation and the availability of the choice-response keys in the standard detection task. In addition, reinforcement sensitivity was lowest when correct choice responses were intermittently, rather than continuously, reinforced. 相似文献
66.
67.
68.
Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Reaction time to detect a phoneme target in a sentence was found to be faster when the word in which the target occurred formed part of the semantic focus of the sentence. Focus was determined by asking a question before the sentence; that part of the sentence which comprised the answer to the sentence was assumed to be focussed. This procedure made it possible to vary position of focus within the sentence while holding all acoustic aspects of the sentence itself constant. It is argued that sentence understanding is facilitated by rapid identification of focussed information. Since focussed words are usually accented, it is further argued that the active search for accented words demonstrated in previous research should be interpreted as a search for semantic focus. 相似文献
69.
Fazio, Zanna, and Cooper (Dissonance and self-perception: An integrative view on each theory's proper domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1977, 13, 464–479) specified mutually exclusive domains of application for dissonance and self-perception theory and used a misattribution-of-arousal procedure to distinguish between dissonance reduction and self-perception processes. Because their proposed limitations of the domains of the two theories and their use of the misattribution procedure are not directly derivable from earlier statements of either theory, it may be best to regard their analysis as a new theory, rather than as a conciliation of the parent theories. New analyses based on the Fazio et al. results indicate that their data provide an insufficient basis for preferring their theory to earlier versions of dissonance and self-perception theories. Dissonance and self-perception: An integrative view of each theory's proper domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1977, 13, 464–479. 相似文献
70.
A. Daniel Yarmey 《Journal of research in personality》1979,13(4):450-459
This study investigated recognition memory of photographs of the subject's own face. Male and female subjects were photographed as they projected sociable faces, trustworthy faces, and intelligent faces. After deciding which face of 10 best represented each characteristic, and judging which photograph best represented their “real self,” a recognition memory test of poses was given. Half of each sex were tested under intentional learning conditions and the remainder were tested under incidental learning conditions. Females demonstrated superior recognition memory of their own facial projections and, in particular, recalled photographs of their “real self” and “most sociable” self most easily. No differences were found between the two learning conditions. Subjects' recognition performance was not related to their confidence of judgments. The results were discussed in terms of sex differences and the role of self in memory. 相似文献