全文获取类型
收费全文 | 222篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
239篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 57篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 11篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 6篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 4篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有239条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
返回抑制(IOR)是指注意过程中个体对已搜索过的位置进行抑制, 从而在随后该位置上出现目标时发生注意重新定向或反应变慢的过程。本研究使用ERPs技术, 采用线索-靶子范式, 考察了情绪目标辨别任务中IOR的发生过程及二者的神经机制。结果发现, 与无效线索相比, 有效线索下的P1波幅更小, N1波幅更大, 正性、负性面孔N170的波幅比中性面孔大。表明当要求被试做情绪辨别任务时, IOR与对情绪信息的反应之间发生了分离, 表现出情绪加工的优先。这说明IOR和情绪偏向是发生在两种不同神经通路上的适应机制, 也为进一步探究注意与情绪的关系问题提供了来自神经电生理学的证据。 相似文献
92.
93.
《Journal of aggression, maltreatment & trauma》2013,22(1):109-126
Summary This study focuses on the unique trauma histories of incarcerated girls. In particular, this study draws upon data obtained from 100 incarcerated adolescent girls, highlighting areas of similarity to and difference from incarcerated boys, including: overall levels of traumatic violence exposure, exposure to unique forms of traumatic violence, psychological symptomatology, and hypothesized trajectories of involvement in serious delinquent activity. In addition, a case example is presented to illustrate our hypotheses about the trajectories of adolescent girls' involvement in serious delinquent behavior, as well as the prominent role of early trauma histories and repeat victimization in these trajectories. 相似文献
94.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(8):1572-1600
In the current study, we examined how short-term memory for location–identity feature bindings is influenced by subsequent cognitive and perceptual processing demands. Previous work has shown that memory performance for feature bindings can be disrupted by the presentation of subsequent visual information, particularly when this information is similar to that held in memory. The present study demonstrates that memory performance for feature bindings can be profoundly disrupted by also requiring a response to visual information presented subsequent to the visual memory array. Across five experiments, memory for a location–identity binding was substantially impaired following a localization response to a following item that matched the location but mismatched the identity of the memory target. The results point to an important role for action in the episodic integration processes that control short-term visual memory performance. 相似文献
95.
Predicting dating behavior from aggression and self‐perceived social status in adolescence
下载免费PDF全文

96.
Do 14- to 17-month-olds notice the paths and manners of motion events? English- and Spanish-learning infants were habituated to an animated motion event including a manner (e.g., spinning) and a path (e.g., over). They were then tested on four types of events that changed either the manner, the path, both, or neither component. Both English- and Spanish-learning infants attended to changes of manner and changes of path. Thus, infants from two different language communities proved sensitive to components of events that undergird relational term learning. 相似文献
97.
98.
Studies that distinguish among believed memories, believed-not-remembered events (e.g., family stories), and nonbelieved memories (i.e., memories no longer believed to have occurred) typically rely on experimenter provided or overtly elicited events. These methods may mis-estimate the frequency and nature of such events in everyday memory. Three studies examined whether such events would be elicited via indirect cueing. Participants recalled and rated events on autobiographical belief, recollection, and other characteristics associated with remembering. All three event types resulted, but with a low rate of nonbelieved memories. Believed and nonbelieved memories received similar perceptual and re-experiencing ratings, and both exceeded believed-not-remembered events. Lifespan cueing found nonbelieved memories to be most frequent in middle childhood (ages 6–11). Cueing for “events” vs. “memories” revealed that “memory” cues lead to retrieval of a more homogeneous set of events and differences when predicting autobiographical belief and recollection. These studies support the distinction between autobiographical belief and recollection for autobiographical events. 相似文献
99.
100.