全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2129篇 |
免费 | 177篇 |
国内免费 | 48篇 |
专业分类
2354篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 5篇 |
2024年 | 24篇 |
2023年 | 44篇 |
2022年 | 47篇 |
2021年 | 57篇 |
2020年 | 105篇 |
2019年 | 130篇 |
2018年 | 129篇 |
2017年 | 143篇 |
2016年 | 114篇 |
2015年 | 104篇 |
2014年 | 79篇 |
2013年 | 269篇 |
2012年 | 65篇 |
2011年 | 68篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 62篇 |
2008年 | 93篇 |
2007年 | 127篇 |
2006年 | 97篇 |
2005年 | 76篇 |
2004年 | 47篇 |
2003年 | 49篇 |
2002年 | 46篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 23篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有2354条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
This article examines the ways in which representational and identity transformations work during the socialization in the workplace of young LGBT hires, linked to discrimination perceived in their workplace environment as well as their impact on professional goals. Working within the theoretical framework of active socialization, the study is based on the IMIS method and proposes a detailed analysis of two cases recently entering the transportation workplace. The results show that, depending on how severe the perceived discrimination is in the workplace, previous experiences of homophobia encountered in other environments and already built self-image are reactivated in different ways, leading to a more or less important reconstruction of professional and identity representations, as well as the future goals of the individual. These elements are discussed from the point of view of interactions between environments and possible interventions by valuing diversity and accompanying the integration of young hires into organizations. 相似文献
82.
运用情景模拟和角色扮演的方法,研究大学生对网瘾和作弊的责任归因结构及其应对策略。对416名被试的研究结果显示:(1)责任归因的心理结构是一个“责任推断→责任情感→责任行为”的一元结构模式。(2)我国大学生的责任推断过程与维纳的经典理论基本一致,但在行为应对策略上存在一定的文化差异。(3)大学生对网瘾和作弊的责任推断倾向明显并与情感体验基本保持一致,但与行为应对策略存在一定的冲突。(4)大学生在网瘾和作弊的责任推断、情感体验以及应对策略上存在一定差异。 相似文献
83.
Bibace R 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2008,42(1):76-86
My response to Engelmann (2008) will be based on several questions that will allow both its author and the general reader to determine whether the assumptions I make as an interpreter of this complex paper are congruent or incongruent with their own interpretations of the text. The interpretations by the writer, by any commentator, and the diverse interpretations of a general audience together with my own interpretations will, I hope, facilitate some fruitful ‘comparative evaluations.’ I articulate my inferences of the most dense part of the paper, namely the ‘concrete immediate Consciousness and the developing absent outside.’ My hope is to address Engelmann’s question: “Am I in a better disposition to judge modern theories of consciousness?” The last section of my response spells out more personal comments to my all too brief and single encounter with Arno Engelmann. It is there that Arno Engelmann’s fascinating statement “I am a citizen of the world” is addressed through its counterparts in my life. 相似文献
84.
John Martin Fischer 《The Journal of Ethics》2008,12(2):167-189
I seek to reply to the thoughtful and challenging papers by Helen Steward, Saul Smilansky, and John Perry. Steward argues
that agency itself requires access to alternative possibilities; I attempt to motivate my denial of this view. I believe that
her view here is no more plausible than the view (which she rejects) that it is unfair to hold someone morally responsible,
unless he has genuine access to alternative possibilities. Smilansky contends that compatibilism is morally shallow, and that
we can see this from the “ultimate perspective.” In reply, I explore the nature of “zooming” arguments, and I contend that
even from a somewhat more detached perspective, important features that distinguish us from mere animals can be discerned
(even in a causally deterministic universe). Finally, I seek to address Perry’s defense of classical compatibilism. My main
objection to his form of compatbilism is that agents must be construed as having a certain kind of “baggage”—even on his own
account.
相似文献
John Martin FischerEmail: |
85.
从系统论看肿瘤的生物靶向性治疗 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
邓远美 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2008,29(1):66-67
肿瘤的生物靶向性治疗是恶性肿瘤治疗的新方法,其试图通过在基因水平上调节与肿瘤发生、发展有密切联系的生命物质来达到治愈肿瘤的目的。但临床运用中,生物靶向性治疗的疗效与我们的想象相去甚远。本文运用系统论的观点,分析目前生物靶向性治疗中存在的问题及寻求可能解决的方法。 相似文献
86.
医药回扣是最大的医患利益冲突 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
雷锦程 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2006,27(10):15-18
分析医患利益冲突如何影响医生的临床决策,并在此基础上探讨如何处理医患利益冲突。医患利益冲突影响医生临床决策有两个环节,其一是影响其专业判断,其二是影响其医疗决定。医药回扣具备商业贿赂的典型特征。医药回扣是最大的医患利益冲突。 相似文献
87.
88.
2003年诺贝尔医学奖给科学界的多重启示(上) 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
2003年诺贝尔医学奖授予了两位美英物理学家,劳特布尔和曼斯菲尔德,表彰他们在磁共振成像方面的发现.这些发现导致现代磁共振技术的开发,这一技术标志着医学诊断学和科学研究中的一项突破.该项发现建筑在核磁和核磁共振现象被发现的基础之上.核磁现象的发现及其应用导致了1943、1944和1952年诺贝尔物理学奖的获得.核磁共振现象的发现及其应用导致了1991年和2002年诺贝尔化学奖的获得.这意味着,2003年诺贝尔医学奖是该系列发现中第六个诺贝尔科学奖. 相似文献
89.
《The journal of positive psychology》2013,8(5):444-452
The present study examined the relationship of meaning in life with emotional distress, suicidal ideation, and life functioning in a sample of 273 active duty Security Forces personnel assigned to two US Air Force bases. Results of regression analyses indicated that stronger meaning in life was significantly associated with less severe emotional distress (p?<?0.001, ΔR 2?=?0.047) and suicidal ideation (p?=?0.043, ΔR 2?=?0.017), and better functioning at work and in intimate relationships, nonfamily relationships, and recreational activities (p?<?0.001, ΔR 2?=?0.073). Meaning in life showed stronger associations with outcomes relative to other predictors and covariates and explained the relationship between belonging and life functioning. Findings suggest that meaning in life is associated with less emotional distress and suicide risk, and greater success and performance across multiple domains in life among military personnel. 相似文献
90.
《The journal of positive psychology》2013,8(2):167-178
Growing popular interest in positive psychology may have important implications for the measurement of well-being. Five studies tested the prediction that well-being ratings are influenced by desirability bias. In Study 1, participants (N?=?176) instructed to fake good endorsed higher well-being; those instructed to fake bad endorsed lower well-being, compared to controls. In Studies 2 and 3 (N’s?=?111, 121), control participants endorsed higher levels of well-being compared to those attached to a bogus pipeline. These differences were mediated by desirability bias. In Study 4 (N?=?417), instruction manipulations did not affect well-being levels, but presenting a desirability measure prior to well-being measures attenuated the correlations between them. In Study 5 (N?=?391), however, this order effect did not replicate. We discuss the importance of continued vigilance for desirability bias in well-being research as a ready solution to this clear problem remains elusive. 相似文献