首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1563篇
  免费   184篇
  国内免费   56篇
  1803篇
  2025年   7篇
  2024年   27篇
  2023年   34篇
  2022年   43篇
  2021年   55篇
  2020年   85篇
  2019年   98篇
  2018年   65篇
  2017年   104篇
  2016年   70篇
  2015年   72篇
  2014年   69篇
  2013年   230篇
  2012年   48篇
  2011年   53篇
  2010年   42篇
  2009年   47篇
  2008年   53篇
  2007年   62篇
  2006年   61篇
  2005年   53篇
  2004年   54篇
  2003年   42篇
  2002年   43篇
  2001年   35篇
  2000年   26篇
  1999年   20篇
  1998年   26篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   19篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   18篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   2篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   6篇
  1975年   7篇
排序方式: 共有1803条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
871.
    
In the field of eye tracking, scanpath analysis can reflect the sequential and temporal properties of the cognitive process. However, the advantages of scanpath analysis have not yet been utilized in the study of risky decision making. We explored the methodological applicability of scanpath analysis to test models of risky decision making by analyzing published data from the eye‐tracking studies of Su et al. (2013); Wang and Li (2012), and Sun, Rao, Zhou, and Li (2014). These studies used a proportion task, an outcome‐matched presentation condition, and a multiple‐play condition as the baseline for comparison with information search and processing in the risky decision‐making condition. We found that (i) the similarity scores of the intra‐conditions were significantly higher than those of the inter‐condition; (ii) the scanpaths of the two conditions were separable; and (iii) based on an inspection of typical trials, the patterns of the scanpaths differed between the two conditions. These findings suggest that scanpath analysis is reliable and valid for examining the process of risky decision making. In line with the findings of the three original studies, our results indicate that risky decision making is unlikely to be based on a weighting and summing process, as hypothesized by the family of expectation models. The findings highlight a new methodological direction for research on decision making. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
872.
    
This is an author response to commentaries on our original article (Wu, Sheppard & Mitchell, 2016). We abstract two main themes from the commentaries, and they are as follows: (1) What kind of clues in target behaviour allow perceivers to infer target EQ? And related with this, by what process do perceivers infer the target's EQ? (2) Do features of the target group naturally lead to a U‐shaped function, whereby it is easy for perceivers to identify those at the extremes but not those in the middle of the continuum?  相似文献   
873.
874.
    
The stories of people who attempt suicide are insufficiently reflected in suicide research in psychology. This article outlines a method using a visual timeline during the interviews to explore the narratives of suicide attempts. The method was successfully employed in two studies to collect data on the suicidal process from people with prior experience of suicide attempts. The advantages of the method in collecting comprehensive data, building rapport, and fostering participants’ insight are outlined. Strategies that were used to ensure safety of participants are described in addition to the requirements for interviewers seeking to apply this method.  相似文献   
875.
ABSTRACT

The aim of this investigation was to determine whether an academic-specific self-compassion scale would share stronger bivariate relationships with academic-focused variables, act as a stronger unique predictor of university adaptation than a general measure of self-compassion, and moderate the relationship between general and academic resourcefulness. A convenience sample of 422 undergraduates completed measures assessing general learned resourcefulness, academic self-efficacy, failure attributions, academic goal focus, academic resourcefulness, academic self-compassion, and university adaptation. As hypothesized, academic self-compassion shared a stronger relationship with academic resourcefulness than a general measure of self-compassion; academic self-compassion independently predicted university adaptation along with academic resourcefulness and expected GPA; and, unlike general self-compassion, academic self-compassion did not moderate the relationship between general and academic resourcefulness, providing support for academic self-compassion being a type of academic self-control that is “emotion-oriented.” The value of using academic-specific versus global constructs when assessing academic outcomes is discussed.  相似文献   
876.
表皮生长因子受体(EGFR)基因突变的非小细胞肺癌(non-small cell lungcancer,NSCLC)是一类具有独特病理和临床特征的恶性肿瘤。目前由于针对EGFR基因突变阳性NSCLC治疗中TKIs的应用,患者的生存期已超过三年,所以此类患者从诊断到治疗应进行全程管理。首先要进行分子检测,发现EGFR基因突变NSCLC,以避免失去EGFR—TKIs的治疗机会。研究证明,EGFR基因突变NSCLC任何线接受第一代抑制剂治疗,患者疗效及生存获益且耐受性良好。一代EGFR—TKIs耐药后根据失败模式选择后续局部或全身治疗,或根据耐药失败分子机制给予新的分子靶向治疗。对EGFR基因突变NSCLC应实施科学、有序的全程管理。  相似文献   
877.
Educators and researchers who study human learning often assume that feedback is most effective when given immediately. However, a growing body of research has challenged this assumption by demonstrating that delaying feedback can facilitate learning. Advocates for immediate feedback have questioned the generalizability of this finding, suggesting that such effects only occur in highly controlled laboratory settings. We report a pair of experiments in which the timing of feedback was manipulated in an upper-level college engineering course. Students practiced applying their knowledge of complex engineering concepts on weekly homework assignments, and then received feedback either immediately after the assignment deadline or 1 week later. When students received delayed feedback, they performed better on subsequent course exams that contained new problems about the same concepts. Although delayed feedback produced superior transfer of knowledge, students reported that they benefited most from immediate feedback, revealing a metacognitive disconnect between actual and perceived effectiveness.  相似文献   
878.
879.
Described is a space model of developmental process and its projection on a plane—the Diagram of development. The latter is used to interpret the process of human history both on universal scale and that of the twentieth century with Soviet history as an instance. Hence unexpected conclusions are received about the impending social change in the world. Summarized are results of the previous mathematical analysis of the Diagram, the content of qualitative changes in the developmental processes is exposed. Also described are probable effects of the world view change in science when the world is seen as a developmental process. Examined are also the perspectives of the distant future for the humankind as a most rapidly developing part of nature. The questions of the human destiny and the role of man's activities in the Universe are also dwelt upon.  相似文献   
880.
Thomas Jay Oord 《Zygon》2013,48(2):466-477
Theists in general and Christians in particular have good grounds for affirming divine action in relation to twenty‐first‐century science. Although humans cannot perceive with their five senses the causation—both divine and creaturely—at work in our world, they have reasons to believe God acts as an efficient, but never sufficient, cause in creation. The essential kenosis option I offer overcomes liabilities in other kenosis proposals, while accounting for a God who acts personally, consistently, persuasively, and yet in diversely efficacious ways. We can reasonably infer that the love, beauty, and truth expressed in creation derive from divine and creaturely causation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号