全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1939篇 |
免费 | 350篇 |
国内免费 | 660篇 |
专业分类
2949篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 61篇 |
2022年 | 90篇 |
2021年 | 78篇 |
2020年 | 148篇 |
2019年 | 139篇 |
2018年 | 127篇 |
2017年 | 149篇 |
2016年 | 177篇 |
2015年 | 96篇 |
2014年 | 148篇 |
2013年 | 447篇 |
2012年 | 129篇 |
2011年 | 162篇 |
2010年 | 94篇 |
2009年 | 128篇 |
2008年 | 120篇 |
2007年 | 118篇 |
2006年 | 98篇 |
2005年 | 84篇 |
2004年 | 59篇 |
2003年 | 57篇 |
2002年 | 41篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 21篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有2949条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Michael JL 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1979,32(2):265-268
In a two-key concurrent variable-interval schedule (using pigeons), if the reinforcement frequency for one response is held constant while that for the other is increased, the rate of response on the constant key decreases. The immediate reinforcement for key pecking can usually be conceptualized as the change from a condition in which the key light is on and the food hopper light is off to one in which the key light is off and the hopper light is on. The prechange condition is associated with a delay to food of one-half the average interreinforcement interval in effect during this condition. The postchange condition is associated with a delay to food of about .5 seconds. The programming of additional reinforcement results in a decrease in the delay to food associated with the prechange stimulus condition, and thus a decrease in the value of the improvement that results from the change. This would appear to be analogous to a decrease in the amount of reinforcement, and thus sufficient explanation for the decrease in the rate of the response. 相似文献
12.
R O Straub M S Seidenberg T G Bever H S Terrace 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1979,32(2):137-148
Three pigeons learned to peck four colors in a particular sequence, regardless of how these colors were positioned on four response keys and without feedback following each response. This demonstrates that serial learning is possible in subprimate animals. 相似文献
13.
Demand for food on fixed-ratio schedules as a function of the quality of concurrently available reinforcement 总被引:5,自引:5,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Six rats lever pressed for food on concurrent fixed-ratio schedules, in a two-compartment chamber. In one compartment, mixed diet pellets were delivered on fixed-ratio schedules of 1, 6, 11, and 16; in the other, either no food was delivered, or sucrose or mixed diet pellets were delivered on fixed-ratio 8. The number of pellets obtained in the first compartment declined as a function of fixed-ratio size in that compartment in all three conditions, but the decline was greatest overall with mixed diet pellets concurrently available in the other compartment, and least with no food concurrently available. The result is discussed in terms of economic demand theory, and is consistent with the prediction that elasticity of demand for a commodity (defined in operant terms as the ratio of the proportionate change in number of reinforcements per session to the proportionate change in fixed-ratio size) is greater the more substitutable for that commodity are any concurrently available commodities. 相似文献
14.
Carnine DW 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1976,9(2):199-206
Off-task behavior, correct answering, and participation during beginning reading instruction were recorded for two low-achieving first-grade children during two different rates of teacher presentation. A slow-rate presentation (A) was compared with a fast-rate presentation (B) in an ABABAB design. In slow-rate presentation, there was a delay between the children's response and introduction of the next task. In fast-rate presentation, there was no delay. A new teacher taught during the final AB phases, which allowed for a brief replication. Both teachers were reminded on a fixed-interval 90-sec schedule throughout all phases of the experiment to praise the subjects, thus preventing a confounding of social praise and rate of teacher presentation. Fast-rate presentation was accompanied by a lower per cent occurrence of off-task behavior for both Subjects 1 and 2. For Subject 1, correct answering and participation were more frequent during all three fast-rate phases. For Subject 2, correct answering and participation were more frequent during the fast-rate phases after the first reversal. 相似文献
15.
词频效应指语言产生中人们对高频词汇的加工比低频词汇更快更准确的一种现象,它可能发生在语言产生中的不同阶段。对青年人和老年人词频效应的不同特点和加工机制进行比较,可以考察语言产生的认知老化机制。通过语言产生理论可对词频效应的老化进行预测,提出词频效应在个体发展和老化阶段的相对稳定性,分析老化导致词频效应相关的神经基础和加工时间进程的改变。未来研究可进一步分离词频效应与习得年龄效应对语言产生老化的影响,并扩展至神经退行性疾病患者中。 相似文献
16.
支持型领导行为在工作压力模型中的作用机制研究 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
本研究调查了 1 3个企业共 2 1 8位员工 ,探讨了支持型领导行为在角色冲突、人际冲突、缺乏成就与发展 (压力源 )和工作满意感、工作焦虑 (压力反应 )之间的作用机制。研究发现 :(1 )支持型领导行为对工作满意感和工作焦虑有主效应 ,但是由于逆向缓冲的存在 ,支持型领导行为的主效应并不总是对的 ,我们必须在更大的范围中考虑领导行为的效应 ;(2 )缓冲效应的六个假设只有一个被证明 ,但是方向相反 ,即支持型领导行为加剧了由人际冲突造成的工作不满意感 ;(3)两个中介模型的部分关系被证明 ,但是我们很难确定是支持型领导行为的弱化效应还是评价作用。为了更好地说明因果关系 ,在将来的研究中纵向研究设计被认为更为恰当。 相似文献
17.
童年中期同伴关系、同伴关系知觉与心理行为适应的关系 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
本研究以422名小学二、三、四年级的儿童进行为期3年的纵向研究,考察了时间2的同伴关系知觉在时间1的同伴关系和时间3的心理行为适应之间的中介效应。结果表明,自我知觉的社会能力在同伴接纳与社交/领导行为和孤独感之间的中介作用显著,自我知觉的友谊质量在双向互选朋友与社交/领导行为和孤独感之间的中介作用显著。 相似文献
18.
语音回路与阅读理解关系的眼动研究 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
语音回路是工作记忆中的一个重要成分。由于工作记忆在言语理解中有重要作用,所以负责加工语音信息的语音回路可能也和阅读理解有密切关系。但是,迄今为止直接研究语音回路影响阅读理解的研究不是很多,而且得出的结论也不太一致。研究使用眼动分析方法探讨语音回路与汉语阅读理解的关系。研究采用抑制发音和错误干扰相结合的范式考查正常条件下和抑制发音条件下的阅读特点的差别性。结果表明:(1)在一般阅读难度情况下,语音回路会延缓阅读速度;(2)两种阅读条件下都出现了同音效应,即同音字的首次注视时间、总注视时间都比非同音字短;(3)语音在汉语阅读的早期就发挥了作用,而且同音字错误比非同音字错误更容易被恢复 相似文献
19.
Previous research in a water-reinforced instrumental training situation with toads (Bufo arenarum) has shown that performance in both acquisition and extinction is poorer after partial, rather than continuous reinforcement training. In Experiment 1, the performance of a group receiving 24 trials on a 50% partial reinforcement schedule was poorer in acquisition and extinction than that of continuously reinforced groups matched for trials or reinforcements. However, partially reinforced toads extinguished at the same rapid rate as a continuously reinforced group that received training only on the days in which the partial toads received water reinforcement. In Experiment 2, extinction was faster after 10 reinforced acquisition trials than after 30 trials. This evidence suggests that the deleterious effects of partial reinforcement in toads can be explained by a combination of two factors, namely, the distribution of reinforced trials across days and the total number of reinforcements. 相似文献
20.
David J. Foulis 《Studia Logica》2006,84(3):407-424
A Heyting effect algebra (HEA) is a lattice-ordered effect algebra that is at the same time a Heyting algebra and for which the Heyting center coincides
with the effect-algebra center. Every HEA is both an MV-algebra and a Stone-Heyting algebra and is realized as the unit interval
in its own universal group. We show that a necessary and sufficient condition that an effect algebra is an HEA is that its
universal group has the central comparability and central Rickart properties.
Presented by Daniele Mundici 相似文献