全文获取类型
收费全文 | 723篇 |
免费 | 88篇 |
国内免费 | 21篇 |
专业分类
832篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 13篇 |
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 19篇 |
2020年 | 44篇 |
2019年 | 34篇 |
2018年 | 30篇 |
2017年 | 40篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 46篇 |
2013年 | 45篇 |
2012年 | 52篇 |
2011年 | 42篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 43篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 26篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有832条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
品牌个性与消费者自我概念的一致性及其对品牌忠诚的影响 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
品牌个性与消费者自概念的一致性是指消费者在进行品牌选择时,倾向于选择那些品牌个性与其自我概念一致的品牌。品牌个性与自我概念的一致性对消费者的品牌忠诚具有积极的影响。许多研究都证实了这一点。本文主要对品牌个性与消费者自我概念的一致性对品牌偏好的影响做了简要的概述,对并对其未来研究方向进行了展望。 相似文献
232.
McCollough效应在完全黑暗和自然视觉条件下的消退过程很缓慢,而利用同频补色条栅来进行反诱导,能在十几分钟内消除显色效应。消除所用时间与反诱导条栅的亮度、颜色和栅条间距(空间频率)有关。进一步说明导致McCollough效应恢复平衡的主要因素跟网膜刺激方式有关。 相似文献
233.
Human preferences for facial attractiveness appear to emerge at an early stage during infant development. A number of studies have demonstrated that infants display a robust preference for facial attractiveness, preferring to look at physically attractive faces versus less attractive faces as judged by adults. However, to-date, relatively little is known about which traits of the face infants use to base these preferences upon. In contrast, a large number of studies conducted with human adults have identified that preference for attractive faces can be attributed to a number of specific facial traits. The purpose of the experiments here was to measure and assess infant's visual preference via eye-tracker technology for faces manipulated for one of three traits known to effect attractiveness judgments in adult preference tests: symmetry, averageness, and sexually dimorphic traits. Sixty-four infants (28 female and 36 male) aged between 12 and 24 months old each completed a visual paired comparison (VPC) task for one of the three facial dimensions investigated. Data indicated that infants displayed a significant visual preference for facial symmetry analogous to those preferences displayed by adults. Infants also displayed a significant visual preference for feminine versions of faces, in line with some studies of adult preferences. Visual preferences for facial non-averageness, or distinctiveness were also seen, a pattern opposite to that seen in adults. These findings demonstrate that infant's appreciation for facial attractiveness in adult images between the ages of 12 and 24 months of age is based on some, but not all, traits that adults find attractive. 相似文献
234.
235.
Gregory P. Hanley Cathleen C. Piazza Wayne W. Fisher 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1997,30(2):229-237
Previous research has demonstrated that destructive behavior may be reduced through noncontingent presentation of attention when attention is identified as the stimulus responsible for behavioral maintenance. Because it may not always be possible to deliver attention in all situations, we examined the extent to which alternative stimuli that have been identified through a choice assessment would substitute for attention (the functional analysis—based reinforcer) in a noncontingent reinforcement procedure. Prior to treatment, functional analyses demonstrated that the destructive behavior of 2 clients with mental retardation was maintained by adult attention. Next, a stimulus choice assessment identified highly preferred tangible items for the 2 clients. Finally, we compared the effectiveness of two noncontingent reinforcement procedures: continuous noncontingent access to attention and continuous noncontingent access to the tangible item identified in the choice assessment. For both clients, these noncontingent reinforcement procedures reduced destructive behavior. The results are discussed in terms of the clinical implications for the treatment of destructive behavior using functional and alternative stimuli. 相似文献
236.
Koehler LJ Iwata BA Roscoe EM Rolider NU O'Steen LE 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2005,38(4):469-484
We examined the effects of stimulus (reinforcer) variation in several different contexts. In Study 1, we identified high-quality (HQ) and low-quality (LQ) stimuli based on results of a paired-stimulus assessment and examined their effects when available under concurrent-reinforcement schedules for 8 participants. No participants showed preference for the LQ stimuli when compared singly or in a varied arrangement to the HQ stimulus. In Study 2, we identified nonpreferred (NP) stimuli based on results of a single-stimulus assessment and examined their effects when available under single-reinforcement schedules for 3 participants. Results of Study 2 were mixed. One participant's data indicated that the varied presentation of NP stimuli produced a modest improvement in performance over that observed when the stimuli were presented singly. By contrast, a second participant's data showed no facilitative effect for the varied delivery of NP stimuli and that the inclusion of an HQ stimulus in the varied arrangement obscured the reinforcing effects of the HQ stimulus. The 3rd participant's data showed no effect for the varied delivery of NP stimuli but an apparent facilitative effect when an HQ stimulus was included in the varied arrangement, which was attributable solely to the presence of the HQ stimulus. 相似文献
237.
《The Journal of social psychology》2012,152(5):592-607
ABSTRACT The study investigated the impact of victim sexual orientation, perpetrator gender, and participant gender on judgements toward a 15-year-old male victim of a depicted sexual assault. One hundred and eight-eight participants (97 male, 91 female) read a hypothetical scenario depicting the sexual assault of a 15-year-old male victim where the victim's sexual orientation and the perpetrator's gender were varied between subjects. Participants then completed a questionnaire assessing their attributions toward both the victim and the perpetrator. Results revealed that male participants blamed the victim more than female participants when the victim was both gay and attacked by a male perpetrator. All participants, regardless of gender, made more positive judgements toward the female as opposed to male perpetrator. Results are discussed in relation to gender role stereotypes and homophobia. 相似文献
238.
Pitchford NJ 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2006,94(4):328-333
Compared with object word learning, young children typically find learning color terms to be a difficult linguistic task. In this reflections article, I consider two questions that are fundamental to investigations into the developmental acquisition of color terms. First, I consider what constrains color term acquisition and how stable these constraints are over culture and age. I review recent studies that have identified conceptual, attentional, and linguistic constraints that seemingly operate on the acquisition of color terms compared with object words and the differential acquisition of color terms within a given language. Second, I consider whether these constraints are specific to the acquisition of color terms or whether they reflect more general constraints that operate on other classes of lexical terms such as different abstract property terms. 相似文献
239.
Female zebra finches show a preference for male zebra finches over heterospecific males based solely on the auditory cues
of males, such as songs. The present study was designed to investigate whether females show a similar preference for male
zebra finches based solely on visual cues. Using a Y-maze apparatus, social preference of female zebra finches was studied
between male zebra finches and male Bengalese finches in three experiments. In experiment 1, where female zebra finches could
see and hear live male zebra finches and male Bengalese finches, the females preferred to associate with the male zebra finches.
In experiment 2, using a sound-attenuated experimental apparatus, subjects could see, but not hear, male zebra finches and
male Bengalese finches. The subjects did not show a significant preference for associating with zebra finches. In experiment 3,
as in experiment 2, females could see live male zebra finches and male Bengalese finches in the sound-attenuated chambers.
However, in experiment 3, the subjects also heard prerecorded auditory cues (i.e., songs and calls) of male zebra finches,
which were presented simultaneously in both arms of the maze. Although the females could not use the auditory cues to identify
the location of the male zebra finches, they preferred to associate with the male zebra finches rather than the male Bengalese
finches. These results suggest that visual cues alone were effective in initiating choice behaviors by females and that auditory
cues facilitate such visually based choice behaviors.
Electronic Publication 相似文献
240.
Huan Cheng;Weili Zheng;Jinfan Zhou;Guanglei Zhang;Shuangshuang Tang; 《Psychologie appliquee》2024,73(3):1158-1187
Advances in information and communication technologies have made organizations more efficient and flexible. However, such technologies have changed how employees work and blur the lines between work and nonwork domains. Drawing on theories of self-regulation and moral disengagement, this study investigates how and when work-related electronic communication during nonwork time leads to employees' negative behaviors. Using two daily survey across two consecutive weeks, we found that work-related electronic communication during nonwork time increased the level of employees' self-regulation depletion and subsequent moral disengagement, resulting in increased workplace deviance the next day. Moreover, segmentation preference amplified the serial indirect effect of work-related electronic communication during nonwork time on workplace deviance (via self-regulation depletion and moral disengagement). Our findings extend the research on work-related electronic communication during nonwork time by offering further insights into its behavioral consequences. 相似文献