全文获取类型
收费全文 | 279篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
289篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 7篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 32篇 |
2012年 | 17篇 |
2011年 | 21篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有289条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
创造性思维是创造性的核心。近年来,脑电和脑成像技术的发展为研究创造性思维的神经基础提供了有力的技术支持,对创造性思维的脑机制研究取得了较大进展。创造性思维的脑机制研究主要包括顿悟的脑机制、发散性思维的脑机制、远距离联想的脑机制、言语创造性和图画创造性的对比的脑机制研究。研究结果显示创造性思维需要多个脑区的参与,因不同的认知任务其关键脑区而有所不同。对创造性思维的脑机制研究进行了总结,对这些研究可能存在创造性思维究竟应该定义为“领域一般的”还是“领域特殊的”的解释进行了讨论,并指出了研究中可能存在的基线任务设置问题和未来研究中需要在研究手段、研究设计、研究领域等方面加以改进的建议 相似文献
212.
Michael L. Anderson 《Synthese》2007,159(3):329-345
The massive redeployment hypothesis (MRH) is a theory about the functional topography of the human brain, offering a middle
course between strict localization on the one hand, and holism on the other. Central to MRH is the claim that cognitive evolution
proceeded in a way analogous to component reuse in software engineering, whereby existing components—originally developed
to serve some specific purpose—were used for new purposes and combined to support new capacities, without disrupting their
participation in existing programs. If the evolution of cognition was indeed driven by such exaptation, then we should be
able to make some specific empirical predictions regarding the resulting functional topography of the brain. This essay discusses
three such predictions, and some of the evidence supporting them. Then, using this account as a background, the essay considers
the implications of these findings for an account of the functional integration of cognitive operations. For instance, MRH
suggests that in order to determine the functional role of a given brain area it is necessary to consider its participation
across multiple task categories, and not just focus on one, as has been the typical practice in cognitive neuroscience. This
change of methodology will motivate (even perhaps necessitate) the development of a new, domain-neutral vocabulary for characterizing
the contribution of individual brain areas to larger functional complexes, and direct particular attention to the question
of how these various area roles are integrated and coordinated to result in the observed cognitive effect. Finally, the details
of the mix of cognitive functions a given area supports should tell us something interesting not just about the likely computational
role of that area, but about the nature of and relations between the cognitive functions themselves. For instance, growing
evidence of the role of “motor” areas like M1, SMA and PMC in language processing, and of “language” areas like Broca’s area
in motor control, offers the possibility for significantly reconceptualizing the nature both of language and of motor control. 相似文献
213.
Dana W. Moore Rafeeque A. Bhadelia Rebecca L. Billings Carl Fulwiler Kenneth M. Heilman Kenneth M.J. Rood David A. Gansler 《Brain and cognition》2009,70(3):267-272
Background/hypothesis
Divergent thinking is an important measurable component of creativity. This study tested the postulate that divergent thinking depends on large distributed inter- and intra-hemispheric networks. Although preliminary evidence supports increased brain connectivity during divergent thinking, the neural correlates of this characteristic have not been entirely specified. It was predicted that visuospatial divergent thinking would correlate with right hemisphere white matter volume (WMV) and with the size of the corpus callosum (CC).Methods
Volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analyses and the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT) were completed among 21 normal right-handed adult males.Results
TTCT scores correlated negatively with the size of the CC and were not correlated with right or, incidentally, left WMV.Conclusions
Although these results were not predicted, perhaps, as suggested by Bogen and Bogen (1988), decreased callosal connectivity enhances hemispheric specialization, which benefits the incubation of ideas that are critical for the divergent-thinking component of creativity, and it is the momentary inhibition of this hemispheric independence that accounts for the illumination that is part of the innovative stage of creativity. Alternatively, decreased CC size may reflect more selective developmental pruning, thereby facilitating efficient functional connectivity. 相似文献214.
Paul J. Rosen Kirsten D. Leaberry Kelly Slaughter Nicholas D. Fogleman Danielle M. Walerius Richard E.A. Loren Jeffery N. Epstein 《Cognitive and behavioral practice》2019,26(3):522-534
Deficient emotion regulation is a common and impairing area of difficulty among children with ADHD. Few interventions specifically address deficient emotion regulation. The Managing Frustration for Children With ADHD (MFC) group treatment was developed to specifically target deficient emotion regulation deficits common to children with ADHD. The MFC was developed as a 12-week multisystemic intervention for emotion regulation deficits among children with ADHD. An open trial assessed the effectiveness of the MFC as an adjunctive treatment for deficient emotion regulation among children with ADHD. Fifty-two children with ADHD ages 9–11 (42 boys, 10 girls) were enrolled in the MFC, with 44 completing treatment. The majority (71.2%) of participants had at least one comorbid internalizing, externalizing, or learning disorder. Intent-to-treat repeated-measures ANCOVA suggested significant decreases in emotion regulation deficits, mood difficulties, and externalizing difficulties following completion of treatment. More than half (53%) of children who completed treatment experienced reliable and clinically significant improvement in at least one area of functioning. The MFC demonstrated promising initial effectiveness in addressing the emotion regulation deficits of children with ADHD. 相似文献
215.
Laura Torrecillas-Martínez Andrés Catena Francisco O’Valle Miguel Padial-Molina Pablo Galindo-Moreno 《International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology》2019,19(2):115-123
Background/Objective:To study pain-brain morphometry associations as a function of post-surgery stages (anesthesia, pain and analgesia) in an acute pain model. Method:Impacted mandible third molar were extracted. Before surgery, an anatomical T1 scan was obtained. Regional brain volumen and subcortical nuclei shapes were obtained. Statistical analyses were done using multiple regression, being pain scores the predictors and voxel volumes, subcortical nuclei volumes and subcortical nuclei shapes, the outcomes. Results:Pain was significantly larger at pain than at anesthesia and analgesia stages, and was higher during anesthesia than during analgesia. Pain intensity was related to grey matter in several cortical (Insula, Mid Frontal and Temporal Gyruses, Precuneus, Anterior Cingulate), and subcortical nuclei (Hippocampus, Thalamus, Putamen, Amygdala), depending of the post-surgical stage. A larger number of brain areas showed significance at pain that at anesthesia and analgesia stages. Conclusions:The relationships of regional brain volumes and subcortical nuclei shapes with pain scores seemed to be unsteady, as they changed with the patient's actual pain stage. 相似文献
216.
A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking 总被引:7,自引:2,他引:5
This article proposes a framework for theory and research on risk-taking that is informed by developmental neuroscience. Two fundamental questions motivate this review. First, why does risk-taking increase between childhood and adolescence? Second, why does risk-taking decline between adolescence and adulthood? Risk-taking increases between childhood and adolescence as a result of changes around the time of puberty in the brain’s socio-emotional system leading to increased reward-seeking, especially in the presence of peers, fueled mainly by a dramatic remodeling of the brain’s dopaminergic system. Risk-taking declines between adolescence and adulthood because of changes in the brain’s cognitive control system—changes which improve individuals’ capacity for self-regulation. These changes occur across adolescence and young adulthood and are seen in structural and functional changes within the prefrontal cortex and its connections to other brain regions. The differing timetables of these changes make mid-adolescence a time of heightened vulnerability to risky and reckless behavior. 相似文献
217.
Calderón-Garcidueñas L Mora-Tiscareño A Ontiveros E Gómez-Garza G Barragán-Mejía G Broadway J Chapman S Valencia-Salazar G Jewells V Maronpot RR Henríquez-Roldán C Pérez-Guillé B Torres-Jardón R Herrit L Brooks D Osnaya-Brizuela N Monroy ME González-Maciel A Reynoso-Robles R Villarreal-Calderon R Solt AC Engle RW 《Brain and cognition》2008,68(2):117-127
Exposure to air pollution is associated with neuroinflammation in healthy children and dogs in Mexico City. Comparative studies were carried out in healthy children and young dogs similarly exposed to ambient pollution in Mexico City. Children from Mexico City (n: 55) and a low polluted city (n:18) underwent psychometric testing and brain magnetic resonance imaging MRI. Seven healthy young dogs with similar exposure to Mexico City air pollution had brain MRI, measurement of mRNA abundance of two inflammatory genes cyclooxygenase-2, and interleukin 1 beta in target brain areas, and histopathological evaluation of brain tissue. Children with no known risk factors for neurological or cognitive disorders residing in a polluted urban environment exhibited significant deficits in a combination of fluid and crystallized cognition tasks. Fifty-six percent of Mexico City children tested showed prefrontal white matter hyperintense lesions and similar lesions were observed in dogs (57%). Exposed dogs had frontal lesions with vascular subcortical pathology associated with neuroinflammation, enlarged Virchow-Robin spaces, gliosis, and ultrafine particulate matter deposition. Based on the MRI findings, the prefrontal cortex was a target anatomical region in Mexico City children and its damage could have contributed to their cognitive dysfunction. The present work presents a groundbreaking, interdisciplinary methodology for addressing relationships between environmental pollution, structural brain alterations by MRI, and cognitive deficits/delays in healthy children. 相似文献
218.
DavidL Robinson 《Personality and individual differences》1998,25(6):319
This report describes empirical tests of the theory that females have higher levels of behavioural and cerebral arousal than males and that these differences cause higher neuroticism scores in females, as well as lower female psychoticism scores and lower scores for females on the Gsar factor of intelligence. Data were obtained from 76 subjects, with equal numbers of males and females and with all adult ages represented. EEG responses to auditory stimulation were quantified in terms of the ‘amplitude, ‘rate of change of amplitude and ‘sweep variability factors obtained from a principal components analysis (PCA). Gf, Gc and Gsar intelligence scores were obtained from a PCA of sub-tests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale plus Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices test. Personality differences were evaluated using the Neuroticism and Psychoticism scales of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ). Bivariate hypotheses relating sex to the arousal, intelligence and personality variables were confirmed. A PCA demonstrated that sex, neuroticism, Gsar, wakefulness and high frequency EEG activity all have high loadings on a common factor. These results are consistent with expectations generated by the cerebral arousability theory of personality and intelligence. 相似文献
219.
Bookheimer S 《Neuropsychology review》2007,17(2):145-155
Patients with lesions in or near eloquent cortex typically undergo one of several invasive techniques to prevent loss of function
following surgery. One of the most promising potential clinical applications of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
is to map these functions as part of the pre-surgical work-up to identify patients at-risk, guide the surgical entry, or tailor
the surgical procedure to prevent deficits. While motor and sensory mapping are relatively straightforward, language mapping
is far more complex. The language system is variable in location across individuals and in many cases may reorganize partially
or completely to the contralateral hemisphere. In addition, multiple regions of the brain contribute to language functioning
including essential regions that must not be removed in surgery, and contributory regions that may result in transient or
insignificant impairments post-surgery. Despite these challenges, an increasing number of studies have supported the use of
fMRI for pre-surgical language mapping in a variety of disorders. This article reviews the literature from three disorders
for which patients benefit from preoperative language mapping: epilepsy, brain tumors, and arteriovenous malformations. Each
disorder presents unique challenges to language mapping. Specific case studies are presented highlighting the both the potential
benefits of preclinical fMRI for language mapping as well as the potential risks and pitfalls. 相似文献
220.
Mary Jo Rattermann Lee Spector Jordan Grafman Harvey Levin Harriet Harward 《Cognitive Science》2001,25(6):941-975