全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2738篇 |
免费 | 520篇 |
国内免费 | 107篇 |
专业分类
3365篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 39篇 |
2023年 | 49篇 |
2022年 | 53篇 |
2021年 | 103篇 |
2020年 | 107篇 |
2019年 | 161篇 |
2018年 | 115篇 |
2017年 | 145篇 |
2016年 | 126篇 |
2015年 | 95篇 |
2014年 | 132篇 |
2013年 | 440篇 |
2012年 | 112篇 |
2011年 | 173篇 |
2010年 | 100篇 |
2009年 | 143篇 |
2008年 | 154篇 |
2007年 | 108篇 |
2006年 | 94篇 |
2005年 | 95篇 |
2004年 | 97篇 |
2003年 | 81篇 |
2002年 | 80篇 |
2001年 | 51篇 |
2000年 | 49篇 |
1999年 | 31篇 |
1998年 | 26篇 |
1997年 | 31篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 23篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 18篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 23篇 |
1976年 | 14篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有3365条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
Peripheral vision outside the focus of attention may rely on summary statistics. We used a gaze-contingent paradigm to directly test this assumption by asking whether search performance differed between targets and statistically-matched visualizations of the same targets. Four-object search displays included one statistically-matched object that was replaced by an unaltered version of the object during the first eye movement. Targets were designated by previews, which were never altered. Two types of statistically-matched objects were tested: One that maintained global shape and one that did not. Differences in guidance were found between targets and statistically-matched objects when shape was not preserved, suggesting that they were not informationally equivalent. Responses were also slower after target fixation when shape was not preserved, suggesting an extrafoveal processing of the target that again used shape information. We conclude that summary statistics must include some global shape information to approximate the peripheral information used during search. 相似文献
952.
Matthew Mundy E. Andrea Sadusky 《Advances in cognitive psychology / University of Finance and Management in Warsaw》2014,10(2):39-48
Individuals with body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) appear to possess abnormalities
in the way they observe and discriminate visual information. A pre-occupation
with perceived defects in appearance has been attributed to a local visual
processing bias. We studied the nature of visual bias in individuals who may be
at risk of developing BDD – those with high body image concerns (BICs) – by
using inverted stimulus discrimination. Inversion disrupts global, configural
information in favor of local, feature-based processing. 40 individuals with
high BIC and 40 low BIC controls performed a discrimination task with upright
and inverted faces, bodies, and scenes. Individuals with high BIC discriminated
inverted faces and bodies faster than controls, and were also more accurate when
discriminating inverted bodies and scenes. This reduction in inversion effect
for high BIC individuals may be due to a stimulus-general local, detail-focused
processing bias, which may be associated with maladaptive fixation on small
features in their appearance. 相似文献
953.
《Psychology of sport and exercise》2014,15(3):255-259
ObjectivesThis study examined the association between physical activity level and primitive cognitive processing during a face recognition task in young adults, a topic that has received little attention.DesignCross-sectional.MethodsThe face recognition task required participants to respond to famous faces but not respond to unfamiliar faces. Task performance and several occipito-temporal event-related brain potentials reflecting the various stages of face processing, from perceptual encoding (N170) to recognition (N250 and face-N400), were assessed during the face recognition task.ResultsAlthough analyses revealed no significant group differences in behavioral performance measures, neuroelectric data showed different time courses of face recognition processes between groups. Active individuals exhibited larger N250 amplitude, reflecting an early stage of facial recognition, for famous relative to unfamiliar faces, whereas inactive individuals did not exhibit such a difference.ConclusionsThese findings are suggestive of a possible association between physical activity and relatively early, primitive cognitive processes. 相似文献
954.
A A Wright 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1978,29(2):261-266
Equal-hue discriminability steps for the pigeon are shown as tabular entries that can be summed or interpolated to produce sequences of equal discriminability steps of various step size. Equal-hue discriminability sequences can be constructed where the number of stimuli and spectral range are specified, or where an interval in one spectral region is to be equated to an interval in another spectral region. 相似文献
955.
Complex learning and information processing by pigeons: a critical analysis 总被引:2,自引:19,他引:2
下载免费PDF全文

Three models of conditional discrimination learning by pigeons are described: stimulus configuration learning, the multiple-rule model, and concept learning. A review of the literature reveals that true concept learning is not characteristic of the behavior of pigeons in matching-to-sample, oddity-from-sample, or symbolic matching studies. Instead, pigeons learn a set of sample-specific SD rules. Transfer of the discrimination to novel stimuli, at least along the hue dimension, is predicted by a “coding hypothesis”, which holds that pigeons make a unique, but usually unobserved response, R1, to each sample, and that the comparison stimulus chosen depends on which R1 was emitted in the presence of the sample. Convincing evidence is found that pigeons do code sample hues, but there is little evidence that allows one to infer that the “coding event” must have behavioral properties. Parameters of the conditional discrimination paradigm are identified, and it is shown that by appropriate parametric manipulation, a variety of analogous tasks may be generated for both human and animal subjects. The tasks make possible the comparative study of complex learning, attention, memory, and information processing, with the added advantage that behavior processes may be compared systematically across tasks. 相似文献
956.
When four experimentally naive pigeons were exposed to occasional forward pairings of a keylight followed by a doorlight (that signaled access to a large social space), all subjects began to peck the lit key. In a second experiment, where the keylight either preceded the presentation of the doorlight or was presented independently of it, key pecking was maintained only in the former circumstance. The unconditioned stimulus in these experiments--arrival in the social space--did not elicit pecking. Hence, the conditioned response of key pecking and the unconditioned response of entering the social space differed. This demonstration of autoshaping with a social-space unconditioned stimulus argues against a stimulus-substitution account of the findings. 相似文献
957.
Pigeons were exposed to two different reinforcement schedules under different stimulus conditions in each of two daily sessions separated by 6 hr (Experiments 1 and 2) or in a single session (Experiment 3). Following this, either a fixed-interval (Experiment 1) or a variable-interval schedule (Experiments 2 and 3) was effected in both stimulus conditions. In the first two experiments, exposure to fixed-ratio or differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate schedules led to response-rate, but not pattern, differences in subsequent performance on fixed- or variable-interval schedules that persisted for up to 60 sessions. The effects of reinforcement-schedule history on fixed-interval schedule performance generally were more persistent. In Experiment 3, a history of high and low response rates in different components of a multiple schedule resulted in subsequent response-rate differences under identical variable-interval schedules. Higher response rates initially occurred in the component previously correlated with high response rates. For 3 of 4 subjects, the differences persisted for 20 or more sessions. Previous demonstrations of behavioral history effects have been confined largely to between-subject comparisons. By contrast, the present results demonstrate strong behavioral effects of schedule histories under stimulus control within individual subjects. 相似文献
958.
Pigeons acquired discriminated key pecking between 528- and 540-nm stimuli by either a response-reinforcer (operant group) or a stimulus-reinforcer (autoshaped group) contingency, with other training-schedule parameters comparable over groups. For the birds in the operant group, key pecks intermittently produced grain in the presence of one hue on the key (positive stimulus) but not in the other (negative stimulus). For the birds in the autoshaped group, pecking emerged when grain was intermittently presented independently of key pecking during one key color but was not presented during the other key color. Two independent contingency assays, peck-location comparisons and elimination of differences in reinforcement rate, confirmed the effectiveness of the two training procedures in establishing operant or respondent control of key pecking. After reaching a 10:1, or better, discrimination ratio between key pecks during the two key colors, the birds received a wavelength generalization test. Criterion baseline key-peck rates were comparable for operant and autoshaped groups prior to testing. On the generalization test, performed in extinction, all birds pecked most at a stimulus removed from the positive training stimulus in the direction away from the negative stimulus. In testing, autoshaped "peak" rates (24.5 to 64.9 pecks per minute) were from 33% to 80% higher than rates in the presence of the training stimuli. Respondent peak shift rarely has been reported heretofore, and never this consistently and robustly. These results further confirm the similarity of perceptual processing in classical and operant learning. They are discussed in terms of Spence's gradient-interaction theory and Weiss' (1978) two-process model of stimulus control. 相似文献
959.
Horton GO 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1975,8(3):311-319
The effect of training on the rate of behavior-specific praise for two fourth-grade teachers was investigated within a multiple-baseline design. Training teachers to identify instances of behavior-specific praise on videotaped presentations (discrimination training) combined with instructions to use praise, and audiotape recordings of the teachers' classroom interactions as feedback, increased the rates of behavior-specific praise. However, the effects were restricted to subject-matter areas in which training was conducted. 相似文献
960.
Control by the auditory or the visual element of a compound discriminative stimulus: effects of feedback.
下载免费PDF全文

Groups of pigeons were trained to depress a treadle in the presence of a compound stimulus consisting of a tone and a red houselight (a) to avoid electric shock, or (b) to obtain grain. Immediate, exteroceptive feedback was equated for avoidance and appetitive groups within an experiment, but varied across experiments from elevation of a nonilluminated feeder to darkening of the chamber, termination of the tone, and elevation of an illuminated feeder. Responding in the absence of the compound stimulus postponed its next occurrence. After performance had stabilized, the degree to which each element controlled treadle pressing was determined. Generally, in the appetitive tests, the red light controlled much more responding than did the tone, but in the avoidance tests, the tone controlled more responding than did the red light. 相似文献