首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   158篇
  免费   40篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   7篇
  2020年   14篇
  2019年   27篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   11篇
  2016年   13篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   9篇
  2013年   19篇
  2012年   2篇
  2011年   3篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   5篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   3篇
  2006年   1篇
  2005年   3篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   2篇
  2001年   1篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   2篇
  1980年   3篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
排序方式: 共有199条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
81.
The authors of this paper first summarize some of the theoretical frames put forward to account for allesthesia. Then, pointing performances in a case of visual allesthesia is reported; unexpectedly, pointing behavior for identical targets was different according to hand. The most salient feature was that a same visual target elicited relatively short RTs with adequate pointing using the left hand versus much longer reaction times with allesthesic pointing using the right hand. Discussion is focused on the presented theoretical frames. In view of the results, authors are led to hypothesize that, in sensorimotor cerebral organization, some aspects of signal processing are dependent upon some premotor aspects of response elaboration.  相似文献   
82.
Resumen

Uno de los principales fenómenos que ha estudiado la psicología del testimonio tiene que ver con las transformaciones que ocurren en la memoria del testigo cuando éste, una vez observado el suceso o delito, entra en contacto con otras informaciones sobre el suceso. El fenómeno de «creación de nuevas memorias» (Loftus y Hoffman, 1989) o de «distorsiones sinceras de la realidad» (Loftus, Korf y Schooler, 1988) ha recibido una considerable atención experimental durante los últimos años, así como un importante debatí acerca d los mecanismos psicológicos que pueden explicar este fenómeno. El presente artículo revisa las principales evidencias experimentales que apoyan las diferentes explicaciones alternativas al efecto de la información posterior al suceso.  相似文献   
83.
《Estudios de Psicología》2013,34(3):309-323
Resumen

La presente investigación se inscribe dentro del marco teórico del apego al lugar (Theory of Place Attachment), teoría elaborada por Shumaker y Taylor (1983), y que ha sido reavivada por Giuliani (1991). En ella se evaluará el impacto de diversas variables sociodemográficas, organizacionales y psicosociológicas referentes al apego al lugar. 134 agentes que trabajan en una estructura hospitalaria de la región Centro de Francia respondieron a un cuestionario que está compuesto por tres partes: (a) una escala de apego global al lugar de trabajo; (b) un aparato de preguntas sobre el apego a diversos lugares del trabajo y (c) una escala de satisfacción y de motivación en el trabajo. Los resultados demuestran que los trabajadores se apegan de manera más intensa a aquellos lugares del trabajo que favorecen la comunicación informal. Los dos tipos de elementos que pronostican el apego al lugar de trabajo son: (a) los que dan sentido al trabajo; (b) aquellos que permiten aislarse para superar el estrés por carga excesiva de exigencias organizacionales.  相似文献   
84.
Abstract

This paper provides a criticial interpretation of the theme, point, and methodological status of Adorno’s so‐called negative dialectic. The theme at issue, ‘non‐identity’, comes in several varieties; and the point of Adorno’s dialectic, namely reconciliation, is multifaceted. Exploration of those topics shows that negative dialectic seques into substantive doctrines, including a version of transcendentalism and a claim about deformation. The peculiar methodological status of negative dialectic explains that adumbration. In the appraisive register, my principal contentions include these: Adorno’s transcendentalism makes some sense of the aforementioned deformation claim; and negative dialectic qua method avoids mystery and metaphysical excess.  相似文献   
85.
Abstract

The aim of this paper is threefold. In the first place, I should like to show that Adorno’s philosophy is dependent, to a degree perhaps not always directly recognized in the literature, on a deeply contentious view on the relationship between the mind and the body. In order to show this, I explore and bring out the epistemic and ethical stakes for Adorno’s theory of the relationship between mind and body. Secondly, I move to better articulate precisely what Adorno’s view on the nature of this relationship is. I hold that his position revolves around positing a porous boundary between the domains of the somatic and the cognitive. In closing, I show that Adorno’s account relies on this domain boundary being unidirectionally porous, in that determination of somatic impulses by cognitive content does not seem a live option for Adorno. I go on to note that this smuggles in a dubious position which does a lot of unearned work. The stakes which his implicit account of the body and mind relationship served to secure thereby come once more to look highly vulnerable.  相似文献   
86.
Abstract

Adorno sees experience as intrinsically interpretative. As interpretation requires normative constraints, in order to guide and channel this interpretative engagement, this opens the question of how experience acquires its motivating criteria. If experience is from the first criterially structured, how are these criteria acquired? Moreover, as these criteria are acquired in isolation from experience – as they are the precondition of that experience – are these criteria sensitive to the particularity of the experiences they produce? In order to address these questions, and the problems they threaten to create for Adorno’s epistemology, I look at Adorno’s theory of impulse and cognition. I argue that Adorno grounds the criterial structure of experience and reason in self-preservation. This provides both a motivation and a determining constraint on the criterial structure of experience. It is also a determining influence which is epistemically flexible and compatible with Adorno’s project of tracing the ‘non-identical’.  相似文献   
87.
在扩充样本的基础上进一步探讨集体主义概念内涵,挖掘“集体”的具体指向,明确“集体”构成.研究一通过40个被试的内隐观访谈和质性分析,得到集体主义概念五个方面内容(集体认同、集体优先、联结信念、责任与义务、发生的条件),指出“集体”的具体指向为泛集体和关系集体,集体主义程度与冲突情境有关.研究二通过102份问卷调查和聚类分析,得到三类集体,即关系比较亲密集体(由母亲、亲兄弟/姐妹、配偶、好友构成)、关系一般集体(由认识的人、亲戚、同事、邻居构成)和关系疏远集体(由本地陌生人、国外陌生人构成),并进一步明确了“集体”的具体构成.结论认为:集体主义概念内涵包括集体认同、集体优先、联结信念、责任与义务、发生的条件五个部分;集体主义中的“集体”由泛集体和关系集体构成,其中关系集体又分为关系比较亲密集体和关系一般集体.  相似文献   
88.
Barreto's paper, ‘Requiem for analytical psychology’ utilized Jung's dreams and visions to argue for the obsolescence of Jungian psychology. Its thesis rested upon the theoretical assumptions of Giegerich's psychology as a Discipline of Interiority, which he and Giegerich claim are themselves based in Jung's psychology. Here I argue that that claim is misplaced because it depends upon a fundamental misunderstanding of the nature of Jung's psychological project. I shall further argue that Giegerich's arguments for a Jungian basis to his psychology rely upon misreadings and decontextualisations of Jung's original texts. Finally, I shall attempt to draw attention to the weaknesses and contradictions involved in Barreto's interpretations of Jung's dreams and visions.  相似文献   
89.
IntroductionWhen variable message signs (VMS) or on-board traffic information systems are used, it is essential that while driving, motorists read and understand the information as soon as possible in order to make appropriate decisions to increase road safety and/or facilitate traffic flow. Thus, it is important to investigate the factors that may increase fast reading and comprehension of on-board traffic information.ObjectivesWe examined the influence of the type of message (warnings vs. recommendations), location of the pictogram (top or bottom of the text), type of display device (IPhone, Blackberry, or Tablet) and its position (horizontal or vertical) on drivers’ fast reading and comprehension of on-board messages provided via in-vehicle system. Moreover, we were interested in drivers’ acceptability of in-vehicle system.MethodForty-nine drivers (MMen = 32, 19–65 years) participated to a reading and comprehension task while travelling on a desktop driving simulator. Participants were exposed to two series of 11 traffic messages displayed on one of the three devices. Reading and comprehension times were measured (= milliseconds) for each message. At the end, they had to fill in a questionnaire on their beliefs about on-board traffic messages and in-vehicle system.ResultsDrivers expressed a positive attitude toward on-board traffic messages and in-vehicle system. Reading and comprehension times were of approximately 4 seconds and were longer for warnings as compared to recommendations. The pictogram placed at the top of the text, the tablet and the vertical display device facilitated fast reading and comprehension.  相似文献   
90.
This research aims at suggesting a causal model of risk-taking, following the announcement of another player's gains in a gamble with elementary characteristics. Risk-taking was measured by investment ratio, the amount the participant decided to bet divided by his current total assets. According to Langer's definition [J Perso Soc Psychol 32 (1975) 311-328], the illusion of control was measured by the level of personal success. The results show that the knowledge of another's gain increased risk-taking and the expectancy of personal success. These results validated the causal-model wherein the effect of the knowledge of another's gain on risk-taking was mediated by the expectancy of personal success. The discussion relates mainly to the need for taking into account the fact that gamblers are not isolated decision-makers, but takes account of some social parameters which guide their choices and actions.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号