全文获取类型
收费全文 | 20769篇 |
免费 | 1642篇 |
国内免费 | 317篇 |
专业分类
22728篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 179篇 |
2023年 | 292篇 |
2022年 | 276篇 |
2021年 | 339篇 |
2020年 | 653篇 |
2019年 | 701篇 |
2018年 | 655篇 |
2017年 | 768篇 |
2016年 | 768篇 |
2015年 | 485篇 |
2014年 | 659篇 |
2013年 | 1975篇 |
2012年 | 558篇 |
2011年 | 523篇 |
2010年 | 428篇 |
2009年 | 647篇 |
2008年 | 815篇 |
2007年 | 867篇 |
2006年 | 805篇 |
2005年 | 683篇 |
2004年 | 587篇 |
2003年 | 481篇 |
2002年 | 453篇 |
2001年 | 290篇 |
2000年 | 275篇 |
1999年 | 244篇 |
1998年 | 186篇 |
1997年 | 154篇 |
1996年 | 144篇 |
1995年 | 123篇 |
1994年 | 106篇 |
1993年 | 89篇 |
1992年 | 80篇 |
1991年 | 52篇 |
1990年 | 58篇 |
1989年 | 40篇 |
1988年 | 43篇 |
1985年 | 481篇 |
1984年 | 593篇 |
1983年 | 511篇 |
1982年 | 583篇 |
1981年 | 562篇 |
1980年 | 550篇 |
1979年 | 500篇 |
1978年 | 556篇 |
1977年 | 433篇 |
1976年 | 435篇 |
1975年 | 343篇 |
1974年 | 340篇 |
1973年 | 287篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
Pace of Life and Enjoyment of Life 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Manfred Garhammer 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2002,3(3):217-256
The paper addresses the paradox that people in modern societies believe themselves to be very happy and satisfied with their lives while simultaneously are subjected to increasing time pressure and pace of life. It starts with presenting evidence for time pressure covering objective and subjective indicators. Evidence for Germany is given by comparing time-budgets in 1991/92 and in 1999. Time use among full time employed in Germany and other EU-societies in the 1990s is compared to the US and Japan by means of diary-data. As regards work life, overwork and the gap between actual and preferred working hours are examined. Survey-results on the relationship between work load, time pressure, stress and health are reported.Most of these data support the conclusion that time pressure has emerged as a major social problem. Hence citizens are beginning to consider time prosperity as a dimension of their well-being beyond their consumer wealth.However, this growing pace of life does not reduce life satisfaction and happiness. Here a paradox evolves as increasing time pressure goes hand in hand with increased subjective well-being (SWB) at country level in cross-cultural comparison as well as at individual level.In the third section some answers to this paradox are explored: A first answer is offered through the modernization theory: life satisfaction and QOL are proliferating along with economic growth and living standards. This process at the same time accelerates social life. Hence, the negative effects of time pressure are counterbalanced by various yields of the modernization process resulting in a pattern of "heavy-going" satisfaction.A second explanation is the psychological approach: time pressure fulfils positive functions for mobilizing individual resources. A variation of this argument is that the multiple-choice-society offers a wide variety of attractive options resulting in people engaging in too many activities. Here, the enjoyment of life is equated with taking advantage of every opportunity.However, this paper prefers a third approach based on Simmel's explanation of the ambivalent consequences of modernity. Even where the majority of citizens report high levels of happiness and life satisfaction – which can be explained through "arousal" or "eu-stress" – the need to ease the time-burden of disadvantaged groups and to down-speed work and social life in general is essential. 相似文献
982.
de Azevedo AM 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2000,45(1):75-91
The author first provides her readers with a brief summary of some of Freud's ideas, as found throughout his work, on the notion of 'unconscious'. The notion of unconscious as noun is contrasted to the idea of unconscious as adjective, this latter being proposed as a quality, or a state, ever temporary, dynamic, and subject to the constant changes going on in the individual's internal psychic world, as well as to external conditions. After presenting some considerations, the author then contrasts the Kleinian model of the mind to the Freudian, and Wilfred Bion's contribution is discussed at some length. Within Bion's conception of psychic functioning, the model of 'dream' is highlighted and, in this regard, clarifications are sought regarding Bion's view of the unconscious. To conclude, a brief and superficial approximation to the work of Carl Jung is touched upon, although the author admits to knowing little of Jung's positions. 相似文献
983.
Astrachan GD 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2000,45(3):449-458
This paper attempts to discern a ground for doing depth psychological work in times of disappearance, loss and the absence of the gods. Through the poetry of Friedrich Hölderlin, and to a lesser extent, Rainer Maria Rilke, it seeks to discover the significance of fragmentation and conflagration in the manifestations of both psyche and the post-industrial landscape we inhabit. What does the ‘death of god’ mean for our notions of symbol and self and for the practice of psychotherapy and psychoanalysis? How do we understand our daily clinical work in the context of ruin, holocaust and despair? 相似文献
984.
Svartdal F 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2000,41(4):315-328
The partial reinforcement extinction effect (PREE), defined as increased behavioral persistence following intermittent reward, is considered an important outcome of instrumental learning contingencies, both inside and outside the laboratory. Since adults have a rich experience with situations in which desired outcomes depend on instrumental responding, we asked whether that experience affects judgments of persistence when relevant contingency information is manipulated. Subjects read simple scenarios with information about behaviors generated by high vs. low reward rate, and then judged the resultant persistence of these behaviors under no-reward conditions. Studies 1 and 2 found no evidence that persistence judgments were affected by contingency information in naive subjects. Studies 3 and 4 compared groups with and without explicit knowledge about behavioral psychology and thus tested possible effects of that knowledge for persistence more directly. Judgments in naive subjects were not reliably influenced by reward rate information, but subjects possessing expert knowledge demonstrated judgments that were reliably affected by contingency information. The results indicate that people do not generate generalized knowledge from normal experience with occasional vs. regular reward. Possible explanations and implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
985.
In a sample of Norwegian 14- and 15-year-old students no significant relationship was found between total externality-internality score and level of ability. However, a significant relationship was found between ability and a subscale of locus of control related to degree of belief in the impact of school effort. The analyses of gender differences showed that girls had significantly higher total internal locus of control scores than boys. Boys were, however, significantly more internally oriented than girls on a subscale related to the respondents' general belief in luck, while girls were significantly more internally oriented than boys on a school effort scale. The present study does not support the notion that girls develop an attributional pattern which is more closely related to their abilities while boys may develop a broader attributional pattern. 相似文献
986.
Fereshteh Ahmadi Lewin 《Journal of Aging and Identity》2000,5(3):137-149
By applying results from in-depth interviews with older Iranian Sufis residing in Sweden, this article describes the benefits of the value system of Sufism prizing mystical union above self-realization. As persons conceive of themselves as all forming parts of other-than-self rather than as separate selves, the result is tolerance towards other people's opinions and the vanishing of hostile feelings and competition. Compassion and identification with the whole of creation allows Sufis to avoid the sense of loneliness and exclusion felt by so many immigrants. 相似文献
987.
Andersen D;Danish Committee on Scientific Dishonesty 《Science and engineering ethics》2000,6(1):25-34
In 1992, The Danish Medical Research Council established a national committee on scientific dishonesty with the twofold task
of handling cases of scientific misconduct and taking preventive initiatives. Scientific dishonesty was proven in only five
cases, but in another nine cases lesser degrees of deviations from good scientific practice were found. The experiences from
a total of 24 treated cases indicated that three key areas were at the basis of most of the accusations and the deviations
from good practice: uncertainty about 1) authorship, about 2) rights and duties to use scientific data and about 3) agreements
at the initiation of joint studies. As a consequence guidelines on good practice have been issued on these key subjects.
An earlier version of this paper was presented at a symposium, Scientific Misconduct. An International Perspective, organised by The Medical University of Warsaw, 16 November, 1998. 相似文献
988.
Switankowsky I 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2000,21(6):567-580
Cartesian dualism has been viewed by medical theorists to be oneof the chief causes of a reductionist/mechanistic treatment ofthe patient. Although I aver that Cartesian dualism is one culprit for the misapprehension of the genuine treatment of patients in termsof both mind and body, I argue that interactive dualism whichstresses the interaction of mind and body is essential to treatpatients with dignity and compassion. Thus, adequate medical carethat is humanistic in nature is difficult (if not impossible)to achieve without physicians adhering to a dualistic frameworkin which the body and person is treated during illness. 相似文献
989.
Stempsey WE 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2000,21(4):321-330
This paper is a response to Christopher Boorse's recent defense of hisBiostatistical Theory (BST) of health and disease. Boorse maintains that hisconcept of theoretical health and disease reflects the ``consideredusage of pathologists.' I argue that pathologists do not use ``disease' inthe purely theoretical way that is required by the BST. Pathology does notdraw a sharp distinction between theoretical and practical aspects ofmedicine. Pathology does not even need a theoretical concept of disease. Itsfocus is not theoretical, but practical; pathology's goal is to contribute tothe healing of patients. Pathology, even experimental pathology, is notvalue-free. Not only ``disease' but also such terms as ``nerve' and ``organ'are laden with conceptual values. 相似文献
990.
This paper argues that both the relativist and the pessimist critiques of the idea of progress are inadequate. Progress is defined as increase in global quality of life (QOL). Such QOL is intrinsically subjective, but not relative. It can be reliably measured through life satisfaction-type questions. The World Database of Happiness provides extensive data on social, economic and psychological factors that correlate with overall QOL. They include wealth, health, security, knowledge, freedom and equality. Various statistical data suggest that all these QOL indicators have undergone significant improvements during the last half century, in most of the world. This gives strong support to the thesis that progress objectively occurs. 相似文献