首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1502篇
  免费   142篇
  国内免费   234篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   35篇
  2022年   35篇
  2021年   47篇
  2020年   80篇
  2019年   101篇
  2018年   103篇
  2017年   107篇
  2016年   105篇
  2015年   58篇
  2014年   76篇
  2013年   343篇
  2012年   53篇
  2011年   73篇
  2010年   60篇
  2009年   54篇
  2008年   56篇
  2007年   71篇
  2006年   51篇
  2005年   51篇
  2004年   44篇
  2003年   35篇
  2002年   38篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   32篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   24篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   15篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   2篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   3篇
排序方式: 共有1878条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
There is evidence that upright, but not inverted, faces are encoded holistically. The holistic coding of faces was examined in four experiments by manipulating the attention allocated to target faces. In Experiment 1, participants in a divided attention condition were asked to match two upright flanker faces while encoding a centrally presented upright target face. Although holistic coding was evident in the full attention conditions, dividing attention disrupted holistic coding of target faces. In Experiment 2, we found that while matching upright flanker faces disrupted holistic coding, matching inverted flanker faces did not. Experiment 3 demonstrated that the differential effects of flanker orientation were not due to participants taking longer to match upright, than inverted, flanker faces. In Experiment 4, we found that matching fractured faces had an intermediate effect to that of matching upright and inverted flankers, on the holistic coding of the target faces. The findings emphasize the differences in processing of upright, fractured and inverted faces and suggest that there are limitations in the number of faces that can be holistically coded in a brief time.  相似文献   
982.
983.
Working Memory Capacity as Executive Attention   总被引:22,自引:0,他引:22  
Performance on measures of working memory (WM) capacity predicts performance on a wide range of real-world cognitive tasks. I review the idea that WM capacity (a) is separable from short-term memory, (b) is an important component of general fluid intelligence, and (c) represents a domain-free limitation in ability to control attention. Studies show that individual differences in WM capacity are reflected in performance on antisaccade, Stroop, and dichotic-listening tasks. WM capacity, or executive attention, is most important under conditions in which interference leads to retrieval of response tendencies that conflict with the current task.  相似文献   
984.
张卫东 《心理科学》2002,25(6):691-693
本文阐述了认知神经科学关于选择注意发生在感知觉中枢信息流加工较早阶段以及前扣带皮层(ACC)在丘脑和初级感觉皮层水平调制躯体感觉输入的研究证据,揭示了ACC在感知觉信息加工早期的选择注意控制执行机能,以及实行该机能的神经途径。  相似文献   
985.
辨别作业难度对返回抑制时间进程的影响   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
陈素芬  王甦 《心理科学》2002,25(2):140-143
在返回抑制范式下,利用线索一靶子模式和辨别作业进行了两个实验,探讨辨别作业难度对返回抑制的影响。以大学生为被试。实验一对辨别作业难度采用组间设计;实验二采用组内设计。结果发现,无论是组间比较(实验一),还是组内比较(实验二).复杂辨别作业条件下返回抑制出现时间均比简单辨别作业晚,表明作业难度对返回抑制的时间进程有显著影响。该结果对返回抑制的机制研究有一定的意义。  相似文献   
986.
We recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) as individuals made source monitoring decisions in a paradigm in which the influence of item familiarity and goal relevance could be separately evaluated. Younger and older adults read a list of words and subsequently distinguished these words from foils in a running recognition test in which some foils were repeated after a lag of 6 items, creating familiar lures. Behaviorally, older and younger adults performed equally well in the recognition of study words and the rejection of singly presented foils. However, older adults were more likely to respond to the familiar lures as though they had come from the study list, thus producing the expected group difference in source-monitoring error. For younger adults the ERPs elicited by the targeted study words were maximal at posterior sites and significantly greater than those elicited by either familiar lures or foils. Older adults generated far less differentiated ERP waveforms but with a markedly greater amplitude at frontal sites. We interpret this frontal maximum in the context of poorer source monitoring as suggesting that older adults are more dependent on controlled processes to make discriminations that seem to occur much earlier and more automatically for younger adults.  相似文献   
987.
The present study examined age differences in executive functioning, using an externally cued task-switching paradigm. Two components of task switching were assessed: the ability to maintain and select among task sets (general switch costs) and the ability to switch between task sets (specific switch costs). In contrast to previous findings, we found large age-related differences in specific switch costs, especially when the number of potentially relevant task sets is increased from two to four. Age-related differences in general switch costs were absent when external task cues subserved executive processing in task switching. Generally, the findings suggest that age-related impairments in task-switching components vary as a function of task uncertainty, such as the presence of environmental prompts to behavior.  相似文献   
988.
Long-term effects of covert face recognition   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
Jenkins R  Burton AM  Ellis AW 《Cognition》2002,86(2):B43-B52
Covert face recognition has previously been thought to produce only very short-lasting effects. In this study we demonstrate that manipulating subjects' attentional load affects explicit, but not implicit memory for faces, and that implicit effects can persist over much longer intervals than is normally reported. Subjects performed letter-string tasks of high vs. low perceptual load (Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfomance. 21, 451-468.), while ignoring task-irrelevant celebrity faces. Memory for the faces was then assessed using (a) a surprise recognition test for the celebrities' names, and (b) repetition priming in a face familiarity task. The load manipulation strongly influenced explicit recognition memory, but had no effect on repetition priming from the same items. Moreover, faces from the high load condition produced the same amount of priming whether they were explicitly remembered or not. This result resolves a long-standing anomaly in the face recognition literature, and is discussed in relation to covert processing in prosopagnosia.  相似文献   
989.
The ability of animals to use behavioral/facial cues in detection of human attention has been widely investigated. In this test series we studied the ability of dogs to recognize human attention in different experimental situations (ball-fetching game, fetching objects on command, begging from humans). The attentional state of the humans was varied along two variables: (1) facing versus not facing the dog; (2) visible versus non-visible eyes. In the first set of experiments (fetching) the owners were told to take up different body positions (facing or not facing the dog) and to either cover or not cover their eyes with a blindfold. In the second set of experiments (begging) dogs had to choose between two eating humans based on either the visibility of the eyes or direction of the face. Our results show that the efficiency of dogs to discriminate between attentive and inattentive humans depended on the context of the test, but they could rely on the orientation of the body, the orientation of the head and the visibility of the eyes. With the exception of the fetching-game situation, they brought the object to the front of the human (even if he/she turned his/her back towards the dog), and preferentially begged from the facing (or seeing) human. There were also indications that dogs were sensitive to the visibility of the eyes because they showed increased hesitative behavior when approaching a blindfolded owner, and they also preferred to beg from the person with visible eyes. We conclude that dogs are able to rely on the same set of human facial cues for detection of attention, which form the behavioral basis of understanding attention in humans. Showing the ability of recognizing human attention across different situations dogs proved to be more flexible than chimpanzees investigated in similar circumstances.  相似文献   
990.
以全部报告法和部分报告法为任务.在Kosslyn瞬时记忆模型框架下,探讨当刺激呈现和报告的方式按列进行时,被试注意窗的放置策略及其对瞬时记忆的影响。结果发现。部分报告优势在这种条件下也是存在的;因为长期的学习而导致的阅读习惯对注意窗放置策略的影响是稳定的;同时发现这种策略使得被试在按列进行的报告方式时,全部报告法和部分报告法的成绩都低于按行报告方式。文章还对上述实验发现作出了进一步的讨论。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号