全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4362篇 |
免费 | 43篇 |
国内免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
4414篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 26篇 |
2021年 | 33篇 |
2020年 | 36篇 |
2019年 | 41篇 |
2018年 | 28篇 |
2017年 | 43篇 |
2016年 | 46篇 |
2015年 | 35篇 |
2014年 | 72篇 |
2013年 | 120篇 |
2012年 | 85篇 |
2011年 | 69篇 |
2010年 | 36篇 |
2009年 | 80篇 |
2008年 | 79篇 |
2007年 | 55篇 |
2006年 | 32篇 |
2005年 | 25篇 |
2004年 | 24篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 245篇 |
1984年 | 272篇 |
1983年 | 221篇 |
1982年 | 292篇 |
1981年 | 297篇 |
1980年 | 295篇 |
1979年 | 300篇 |
1978年 | 303篇 |
1977年 | 245篇 |
1976年 | 271篇 |
1975年 | 197篇 |
1974年 | 209篇 |
1973年 | 174篇 |
排序方式: 共有4414条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
171.
172.
173.
174.
Responding to indirect speech acts 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Herbert H. Clark 《Cognitive psychology》1979,11(4):430-477
Indirect speech acts, like the request Do you know the time?, have both a literal meaning, here “I ask you whether you know the time,” and an indirect meaning “I request you to tell me the time.” In this paper I outline a model of how listeners understand such speech acts and plan responses to them. The main proposals are these. The literal meaning of indirect speech acts can be intended to be taken seriously (along with the indirect meaning) or merely pro forma. In the first case listeners are expected to respond to both meanings, as in Yes, I do—it's six, but in the second case only to the indirect meaning, as in It's six. There are at least six sources of information listeners use in judging whether the literal meaning was intended seriously or pro forma, as well as whether there was intended to be any indirect meaning. These proposals were supported in five experiments in which ordinary requests for information were made by telephone of 950 local merchants. 相似文献
175.
Robert E. Remez 《Cognitive psychology》1979,11(1):38-57
Two experiments were performed employing acoustic continua which change from speech to nonspeech. The members of one continuum, synthesized on the Pattern Playback, varied in the bandwidths of the first three formants in equal steps of change, from the vowel /α/ to a nonspeech buzz. The other continuum, achieved through digital synthesis, varied in the bandwidths of the first five formants, from the vowel /æ/ to a buzz. Identification and discrimination tests were carried out to establish that these continua were perceived categorically. Perceptual adaptation of these continua revealed shifts in the category boundaries comparable to those previously reported for speech sounds. The results were interpreted as suggesting that neither phonetic nor auditory feature detectors are responsible for perceptual adaptation of speech sounds, and that feature detector accounts of speech perception should therefore be reconsidered. 相似文献
176.
Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Reaction time to detect a phoneme target in a sentence was found to be faster when the word in which the target occurred formed part of the semantic focus of the sentence. Focus was determined by asking a question before the sentence; that part of the sentence which comprised the answer to the sentence was assumed to be focussed. This procedure made it possible to vary position of focus within the sentence while holding all acoustic aspects of the sentence itself constant. It is argued that sentence understanding is facilitated by rapid identification of focussed information. Since focussed words are usually accented, it is further argued that the active search for accented words demonstrated in previous research should be interpreted as a search for semantic focus. 相似文献
177.
R J Sternberg 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1979,28(3):469-498
A total of 224 subjects in grades 2, 4, 6, 8, high school, and college solved problems requiring comprehension of the logical connectives and, or, if-then, only if, and if and only if, as well as the terms is and is not. Half the subjects were required merely to encode the connectives; the other half were required to combine as well as encode them. Problems were presented in two replications (over two sessions) via two different content vehicles. Quantitative analyses revealed significant effects of task, age, session, and connective, with comprehension of different connectives developing at different rates. In general, comprehension of the conjunctive connective was easiest; comprehension of the conditional and biconditional connectives was most difficult. Qualitative analyses indicated just how the logical connectives were interpreted at each grade level, and also investigated individual differences within each grade level. 相似文献
178.
The present study explores two questions: What is the nature of older children's syntactic knowledge; how is that knowledge used in an everyday speech situation? Six-, eight-, and ten-year-olds repeated grammatical sentences as read by the first experimenter. Half the sentences were syntactically clear, half slightly distorted. Clear versions displayed basic grammatical relations and constituent structure perspicuously. The second experimenter, who sat at the other end of the room, asked “What?” after each sentence. The syntactic changes children might make to accommodate the listener were examined. Although the children made a variety of changes, at all ages they tended to change distorted versions to clear ones, and to repeat clear versions. The results suggest that children's syntactic knowledge is deeper and more accessible than had been supposed. 相似文献
179.
Lynne M Reder 《Cognitive psychology》1979,11(2):221-234
In two experiments, subjects read stories and were asked to make plausibility judgments about statements with respect to the stories. The inherent plausibility of the queried statements, the amount of attention subjects focused on information necessary for making a judgment, and the interval between presentation of the relevant story information and the test probe were varied orthogonally. The pattern of latencies obtained to make these judgments cast strong doubt on the notion that question answering is typically accomplished by searching for a single fact in memory. Rather, people seem to retrieve any relevant, available information and then use this to compute whether a statement seems true. The independent variables in these experiments can be interpreted according to whether they affect the retrieval or the judgment phase. 相似文献
180.
Craig R. Barclay 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1979,27(2):262-276
The present study focused on the executive monitoring of a rehearsal strategy by sixth (11 years 5 months), tenth (15 years 8 months), and twelfth (17 years 9 months) graders and adults (27 years 4 months). The results showed that all of the adults spontaneously adopted an “optimal” strategy and generalized it to a second task. However, none of the sixth graders and only two tenth and three twelfth graders did so. All of the school subjects subsequently trained to rehearse, maintained the strategy without prompting, and showed evidence of transfer. In comparison to adults, the trained subjects' generalized routines became more similar to the adults' as grade increased. This study supports the notion that efficient executive functioning includes the case where continued use of a mnemonic routine is a reasonable response to an objective change in an information-processing task. 相似文献