全文获取类型
收费全文 | 995篇 |
免费 | 75篇 |
国内免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
1086篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 30篇 |
2020年 | 43篇 |
2019年 | 44篇 |
2018年 | 47篇 |
2017年 | 52篇 |
2016年 | 44篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 107篇 |
2012年 | 29篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 59篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 64篇 |
2006年 | 55篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 45篇 |
2003年 | 36篇 |
2002年 | 35篇 |
2001年 | 22篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1086条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Culture wars: American moral divisions across the adult lifespan 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Lene Arnett Jensen 《Journal of Adult Development》1997,4(2):107-121
Sociologists have argued that the public moral debate in contemporary America is characterized by a “culture war,” pitting
“orthodox” and “progressivist” groups against one another (Hunter, 1991). This study addressed whether the culture war is
evident in the moral thinking of ordinary Americans, and whether age-related patterns exist. Sixty fundamentalist Baptists
(orthodox) and 60 mainline Baptists (progressivist) evaluated and reasoned about moral issues such as divorce and abortion.
Each group was divided evenly into three age groups copsisting of young, midlife, and older adults. Moral reasoning was analyzed
in terms of Shweder's (1990) ethics of autonomy, community, and divinity. Within all three age groups, progressivists used
the ethic of, autonomy more than orthodox participants. Orthodox participants used the ethic of divinity more than progressivists.
Orthodox and progressivist groups did not differ much in their use of the ethic, of community. Very few age group differences
were found within the orthodox and progressivist groups. It is concluded that morality is conceived of in markedly different
wasy by orthodox and progressivist groups, and that these conceptions are consistent across age groups. 相似文献
2.
Agnes Verbiest 《Argumentation》1995,9(5):821-836
An incidental extension of the central domain of argumentation theory with non-classical ways of constructing arguments seems to automatically raise a question that is otherwise rarely posed, namely whether or not it is useful to consider the sex of the arguer. This question is usually posed with regard to argumentation by women in particular. Do women rely more, or differently than men do on non-canonical modes of reasoning stemming from the realm of the emotional, physical and intuitive, instead of the logical? One may simply refer this question to folk-linguistics. One may also take the question seriously, given the findings on women's linguistic behaviour, and for various other reasons that will be explained below.Section 1 sums up the most frequently quoted differences in language use between women and men. This is followed by a non-exhaustive, interdisciplinary review of studies on male/female differences in verbal and written argumentation.Section 2 discusses the role of language and texts in generating and maintaining ideas on gender. These gender messages not only influence the actual argumentation behaviour of women and men, but also the way such behaviour is valued.Section 3 subsequently shows that our ideas on rationality are gendered, and therefore also our ideas on the proper central domain of argumentation theory.Section 4 briefly reflects on why this kind of wrong question about the reasoning of women should sometimes be addressed seriously anyway. 相似文献
3.
Moral psychology has long focused on reasoning, but recent evidence suggests that moral judgment is more a matter of emotion and affective intuition than deliberate reasoning. Here we discuss recent findings in psychology and cognitive neuroscience, including several studies that specifically investigate moral judgment. These findings indicate the importance of affect, although they allow that reasoning can play a restricted but significant role in moral judgment. They also point towards a preliminary account of the functional neuroanatomy of moral judgment, according to which many brain areas make important contributions to moral judgment although none is devoted specifically to it. 相似文献
4.
5.
Diversity-Based Reasoning in Children 总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8
One of the hallmarks of inductive reasoning by adults is the diversity effect, namely that people draw stronger inferences from a diverse set of evidence than from a more homogenous set of evidence. However, past developmental work has not found consistent diversity effects with children age 9 and younger. We report robust sensitivity to diversity in children as young as 5, using everyday stimuli such as pictures of objects with people. Experiment 1 showed the basic diversity effect in 5- to 9-year-olds. Experiment 2 showed that, like adults, children restrict their use of diversity information when making inferences about remote categories. Experiment 3 used other stimulus sets to overcome an alternate explanation in terms of sample size rather than diversity effects. Finally, Experiment 4 showed that children more readily draw on diversity when reasoning about objects and their relations with people than when reasoning about objects' internal, hidden properties, thus partially explaining the negative findings of previous work. Relations to cross-cultural work and models of induction are discussed. 相似文献
6.
Mark Fischle 《Political psychology》2000,21(1):135-159
This study attempts to account for the vastly different trajectories taken by mass andelite opinion in the wake of the Lewinsky affair. Data from a panel study, collected before andjust after the scandal broke, suggest that Clinton's prior popularity indelibly colored massresponse to the scandal, thereby constraining citizens' reactions. As would be predicted bytheories of "motivated reasoning," the influence of various considerations (like thecredibility and importance of the allegations) on reactions to the scandal was conditional uponprior affect for the president. Such findings are difficult to accommodate within the more rational"Bayesian updating" perspective. These data shed light on mass response to theLewinsky scandal in particular and citizen reaction to presidential behavior more generally, aswell as on the cognitive mechanisms that facilitate motivated reasoning in candidate evaluation. 相似文献
7.
Clare Conry-Murray Megan McKechnie Nicole Olivieri Pagan 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2020,181(2-3):171-180
AbstractParticipants with ADHD (n?=?45) and participants without ADHD (n?=?130, total n?=?175) judged hypothetical moral and conventional rule violations that varied the impulsivity of the act, the ADHD diagnosis, and the gender of the actor in order to examine (1) social reasoning about impulsiveness and (2) whether participants infer impulsiveness from the characteristics of the actor, including gender and ADHD-status. Moral violations were judged more negatively than conventional violations, even when they were impulsive. The characteristics of the actor influenced judgments in that participants judged boys’ behavior as more acceptable, as having less control, and as deserving of less punishment compared to girls. In addition, actors who were described as having ADHD were judged overall more positively. Participants with ADHD judged that all actors should receive similar punishment, regardless of the actor’s ADHD diagnosis, while participants without ADHD judged actors with ADHD should receive less punishment than those without. 相似文献
8.
In this study with within-subject design, the “be-creative effect” was investigated using three instructions in figural divergent-thinking tasks: Besides a be-fluent instruction, a be-creative instruction, and a be-creative instruction combined with strategies were applied. In addition, reasoning ability as part of fluid intelligence, self-reported creative ideation, current motivation after reading the instructions, and task-related interest were assessed. Multilevel analysis applying random-intercept-constant-slope models found significant main effects of the instructions on creative performance and fluency. Both be-creative instructions yielded a rise in creative performance in divergent thinking tasks with additional improvement due to supporting strategies. The be-fluent instruction enhanced the quantity of ideas but decreased creative performance. Task-related interest turned out to be a better predictor of creative performance than the self-estimated expected interest prior to working on the task. Moreover, the relationship of creative performance and reasoning ability was moderated by instruction, with no relationship with a be-fluent instruction and small to moderate positive effects with both instructions emphasizing creative ideas. 相似文献
9.
David Jenkins 《Ratio》2020,33(2):87-96
Recent philosophical work on the relation between reasoning and bodily action is dominated by two views. It is orthodox to have it that bodily actions can be at most causally involved in reasoning. Others have it that reasoning can constitutively involve bodily actions, where this is understood as a matter of non-mental bodily events featuring as constituents of practical reasoning. Reflection on cases of reasoning out-loud suggests a neglected alternative on which both practical and theoretical reasoning can have bodily actions as constituents, where such bodily actions themselves amount to contentful mental events. Furthermore, the natural lines of resistance to this view trade on type-token errors, or on a questionable common-factor assumption. 相似文献
10.
AbstractExperiencing work-related trauma can negatively impact coping abilities and daily life participation. Occupational therapists (OTs) often provide health services to workers who have experienced trauma, including military members, veterans, and public safety personnel. The Occupational Therapy Trauma Intervention Framework (OTTIF) is a novel clinical reasoning framework that matches a client’s level of readiness to engage with a spectrum of evidence-based OT approaches. Using surveys at two time points, evaluation of therapist experiences of using the OTTIF elicited positive feedback that suggests the OTTIF provides a useful occupation-centred framework for evidence-informed occupational therapy practice with clients who have experienced trauma. 相似文献