全文获取类型
收费全文 | 860篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
872篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 27篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 32篇 |
2016年 | 38篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 93篇 |
2012年 | 40篇 |
2011年 | 45篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 40篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 33篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
1951年 | 5篇 |
1923年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有872条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
232.
Rattanakorn Ratanashevorn;So Rin Kim;Susan Kashubeck-West;Timo T. Ojanen; 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2024,102(3):253-263
Literature suggests LGBTQ+ cultural competencies and cultural humility as critical components to providing counseling services to LGBTQ+ clients. Previous research has shown that cultural humility leads to better counseling outcomes through the counseling alliance in sexual minority clients. This study investigated the relationships among cultural humility, counseling alliance, and counseling outcome among 584 LGBTQ+ counseling clients (38.5% were non-cisgender). The results revealed that all three constructs were positively related to one another. Counseling alliance mediated the relationship between cultural humility and counseling outcome. The overall mediation model indicated that cultural humility had direct and indirect effects accounting for 58% of the variance in counseling outcome when controlling for gender, sexual orientation, and level of education. The findings underscore the importance of cultivating cultural humility among counselors working with LGBTQ+ clients to improve counseling alliance and counseling outcomes. 相似文献
233.
We reflect on family therapy and its history from two points of view, as an entity that becomes understood with the help of a twofold concept of a game, and a twofold concept of liberty. Systemic family therapy has always been comprehended with the help of game theory. Its development becomes more properly understood if we keep in mind that game itself is a dualistic concept entailing both a cultural and a logico-mathematical interpretation of a game. We show how cultural ethos has molded the ways how game metaphor have been implemented to systemic thinking. In the same manner we show how Isaiah Berlin's idea of two incompatible concepts of liberty helps to contextualize family therapy in a way that its connections to sociopolitical theories of liberty become obvious. We believe that we have been able to demonstrate, how this twofold recontextualisation enriches the understanding of the ideological history of the family therapy. We claim that our reflections imply that family therapy is essentially a dualistic endeavor, that in the amid of it is a rift that cannot become repaired but only contemplated, that integrity of family therapy requires that we preserve both conflictual views, and don't try to simplify situation by abandoning one or the other. As a result, our article intends to develop further and deepen the idea that is originally presnted in the article \"Strategy and intervention or non-intervention: A matter of theory\" by Harold Goolishian and Harlene Anderson. 相似文献
234.
The media is increasingly blamed for inflaming political animosity, but it may also bridge partisan divides—with the right strategies. Past research highlights the outgroup-experience effect: Sharing personal experiences (and not facts) helps to reduce partisan animosity. However, sharing facts is a pillar of good journalism and is essential for mediated political communication. Across four studies in two countries, we show that journalists, and citizens on social media sites, can share facts about contentious political issues (gun and climate policy), while simultaneously increasing tolerance and reducing dehumanization of political opponents. We extend the outgroup-experience effect by introducing factual content alongside personal experiences of political adversaries (i.e., a combination approach). These effects are replicated in both the United States and Germany although in Germany the personal experience intervention is only beneficial for people with more extreme attitudes. 相似文献
235.
Sami Yli-Piipari Chee Keng John Wang Timo Jaakkola Jarmo Liukkonen 《Journal of Applied Sport Psychology》2013,25(4):401-417
The study examined the growth trajectories in 757 (M age = 12.71) Finnish middle school students’ intrinsic and extrinsic motivation along with their enjoyment and physical activity. In addition, the study aimed to examine the role of intrinsic and extrinsic physical education motivation in the development in adolescents’ enjoyment and physical activity by identifying adolescents’ subgroups. Adolescents’ enjoyment and physical activity declined, intrinsic motivation was stable, and extrinsic motivation increased across middle school. Adolescents with highest levels of intrinsic motivation and moderate to high levels of extrinsic regulation had the highest levels of enjoyment and physical activity. 相似文献
236.
237.
Kai Kaspar Thea Wehlitz Sara von Knobelsdorff Tim Wulf Marie Antoinette Oktavie von Saldern 《International journal of psychology》2015,50(5):372-378
Text‐based communication is one of the substantial ways of spreading scientific information. While the content and contextual aspects of written words have been widely researched, the impact of font characteristics on text perception is an almost blank page. The following study deals with the influence of serifs on the evaluation of online‐presented scientific abstracts. Yet there is only evidence for faster reading times when texts are presented in sans‐serif fonts, although the opposite is stated in parts of the literature. The present work examines if the presence or absence of serifs also have an impact on the appraisal of scientific texts when all other important font characteristics do not change. For this purpose, 188 university students participated in an online experiment and rated different aspects of scientific abstracts as well as of the research outlined in the abstracts. The results show that missing serifs led to increased reading speed. However, and in contrast to the perceptual fluency hypothesis, the presence of serifs had a positive effect on all evaluation dimensions. The results of a second study with 187 participants also indicated that reading fluency counteracted the liking of texts. Implications for future studies and media production are discussed. 相似文献
238.
239.
Timo Jütten 《Inquiry (Oslo, Norway)》2013,56(3):235-256
In this paper I criticise Axel Honneth's reactualization of reification as a concept in critical theory in his 2005 Tanner Lectures and argue that he ultimately fails on his own terms. His account is based on two premises: (1) reification is to be taken literally rather than metaphorically, and (2) it is not conceived of as a moral injury but as a social pathology. Honneth concludes that reification is “forgetfulness of recognition”, more specifically, of antecedent recognition, an emphatic and engaged relationship with oneself, others and the world, which precedes any more concrete relationship both genetically and categorially. I argue against this conception of reification on two grounds. (1) The two premises of Honneth's account cannot be squared with one another. It is not possible to literally take a person as a thing without this being a recognisable moral injury, and, therefore, I suggest that there are no cases of literal reification. (2) Honneth's account is essentially ahistorical, because it is based on an anthropological model of recognition that tacitly equates reification with autism. In conclusion, I suggest that any successful account of reification must (i) take reification metaphorically and (ii) offer a social-historical account of the origin(s) of reification. 相似文献
240.
Sophie von Stumm 《Cognition & emotion》2013,27(8):1545-1558
ABSTRACTWithin-person changes in mood, which are triggered by situational cues, for example someone’s location or company, are thought to affect contemporaneous cognitive function. To test this hypothesis, data were collected over 6 months with the smartphone application (app) moo-Q that prompted users at random times to rate their mood and complete 3 short cognitive tests. Out of 24,313 people across 154 countries, who downloaded the app, 770 participants submitted 10 or more valid moo-Q responses (mean?=?23; SD?=?18; range 10–207). Confirming previous research, consistent patterns of association emerged for 6 different situation cues with mood and cognitive function: For example, being alone rather than with others when completing the app resulted in worse mood but better cognitive task performance. Notwithstanding, changes in mood and cognitive function were not coupled. The advantages and challenges of using smartphone technology for studying mood and cognitive function are discussed. 相似文献