全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3819篇 |
免费 | 202篇 |
专业分类
4021篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 39篇 |
2022年 | 41篇 |
2021年 | 74篇 |
2020年 | 81篇 |
2019年 | 113篇 |
2018年 | 149篇 |
2017年 | 150篇 |
2016年 | 167篇 |
2015年 | 117篇 |
2014年 | 136篇 |
2013年 | 461篇 |
2012年 | 206篇 |
2011年 | 231篇 |
2010年 | 133篇 |
2009年 | 139篇 |
2008年 | 189篇 |
2007年 | 151篇 |
2006年 | 135篇 |
2005年 | 102篇 |
2004年 | 122篇 |
2003年 | 126篇 |
2002年 | 99篇 |
2001年 | 75篇 |
2000年 | 82篇 |
1999年 | 57篇 |
1998年 | 42篇 |
1997年 | 38篇 |
1996年 | 22篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 39篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 47篇 |
1989年 | 44篇 |
1988年 | 38篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 31篇 |
1985年 | 29篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
1938年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有4021条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
92.
van der Veer R 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》2000,36(2):109-126
In this paper early work of the American rehabilitation psychologist Tamara Dembo (1902-1993) is brought to light. She was highly influenced by the concepts of Kurt Lewin's topological psychology, and she used the framework of topological psychology to analyze her investigations on animal behavior carried out with the Dutch zoopsychologist Frederik J. J. Buytendijk. These investigations have so far been ignored and are being described for the first time making use of archival materials. 相似文献
93.
Hundreds of research articles have addressed the relationship between birth order and intelligence. Virtually all have used cross-sectional data, which are fundamentally flawed in the assessment of within-family (including birth order) processes. Although within-family models have been based on patterns in cross-sectional data, a number of equally plausible between-family explanations also exist. Within-family (preferably intact-family) data are prerequisite for separating within- and between-family causal processes. This observation reframes an old issue in a way that can be easily addressed by studying graphical patterns. Sibling data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth are evaluated, and the results are compared with those from other studies using within-family data. It appears that although low-IQ parents have been making large families, large families do not make low-IQ children in modern U.S. society. The apparent relation between birth order and intelligence has been a methodological illusion. 相似文献
94.
de Courten Manon Schrooyen Pauline Simons Anton van der Zweerde Evert Nethercott Frances 《Studies in East European Thought》2000,52(3):227-237
Studies in East European Thought - 相似文献
95.
Synthese - In clinical practice, decision-making is not performed by individual knowers but by an assemblage of people and instruments in which no one member has full access to every piece of... 相似文献
96.
Synthese - The paradox of phase transitions raises the problem of how to reconcile the fact that we see phase transitions happen in concrete, finite systems around us, with the fact that our best... 相似文献
97.
Gijsbert Bijlstra Désirée Kleverwal Tjits van Lent Rob W. Holland 《Cognition & emotion》2019,33(2):386-389
Recently, Cognition and Emotion published an article demonstrating that age cues affect the speed and accuracy of emotion recognition. The authors claimed that the observed effect of target age on emotion recognition is better explained by evaluative than stereotype associations. Although we agree with their conclusion, we believe that with the research method the authors employed, it was impossible to detect a stereotype effect to begin with. In the current research, we successfully replicate previous findings (Study 1). Furthermore, by changing the comparative context, Study 2 provides a first test of age-stereotypes affecting emotions recognition. We discuss recommendations for future studies in the domain of social categorisation and emotion recognition. 相似文献
98.
Testing the Situationism Scale in Europe: Scale validation,self‐regulation and regional differences 下载免费PDF全文
Megan E. Roberts Lisa Wagner Saša Zorjan Enikö Nèmeth Désie van Toor Michał Czaplinski 《International journal of psychology》2017,52(4):264-272
The term situationism refers to an individual's belief about the importance of a behaviour's context. This study tested whether the degree of situationism expressed by individuals in various regions of Europe was consistent with self‐regulation and cross‐cultural theories. The English version of a Situationism Scale (measuring beliefs about the relation between the environment and one's own behaviour) was translated into five additional languages: Dutch, German, Hungarian, Italian and Slovenian. Young adults (N = 1106, MAge = 22.9 years, 79% female) across Europe responded to one of the six language versions of the scale as part of a larger survey. Results indicated that: new language versions were psychometrically valid; there was a positive relation between situationism and the use of situation‐control strategies; and situationism was higher for individuals from regions that are Eastern European and relatively more interdependent, compared with individuals from regions that are Western European and relatively less interdependent. As the first evaluation of the Situationism Scale outside America, this study supports the Scale's validity and suggests not only may some effects of situationism be universal, but between‐ and within‐culture differences in situationism exist. Overall, when making judgments and decisions about the self, cultural background and individual differences in situationism may come into play. 相似文献
99.
What Does it Take to Break the Silence in Teams: Authentic Leadership and/or Proactive Followership? 下载免费PDF全文
Hannes Guenter Bert Schreurs IJ. Hetty van Emmerik Shuhua Sun 《Psychologie appliquee》2017,66(1):49-77
Leadership may help break the silence in teams, but this may not be equally true for all employees. Using behavioral plasticity theory, we propose that authentic leadership—a set of leadership behaviors through which leaders enact their true selves—reduces silence and motivates speaking up in employees low on proactive personality, but hardly affects employees who are proactive by nature, because proactive employees are less susceptible to social influences. Using data from 223 employees (nested in 45 work teams), we indeed find authentic leadership to reduce silence in employees with less proactive personalities, but not in more proactive employees. We discuss theoretical and practical implications for silence and authentic leadership. 相似文献
100.
Juhani Järvikivi Roger P. G. van Gompel Jukka Hyönä 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2017,46(3):525-550
Two visual-world eye-tracking experiments investigating pronoun resolution in Finnish examined the time course of implicit causality information relative to both grammatical role and order-of-mention information. Experiment 1 showed an effect of implicit causality that appeared at the same time as the first-mention preference. Furthermore, when we counterbalanced the semantic roles of the verbs, we found no effect of grammatical role, suggesting the standard observed subject preference has a large semantic component. Experiment 2 showed that both the personal pronoun hän and the demonstrative tämä preferred the antecedent consistent with the implicit causality bias; tämä was not interpreted as referring to the semantically non-prominent entity. In contrast, structural prominence affected hän and tämä differently: we found a first-mention preference for hän, but a second-mention preference for tämä. The results suggest that semantic implicit causality information has an immediate effect on pronoun resolution and its use is not delayed relative to order-of-mention information. Furthermore, they show that order-of-mention differentially affects different types of anaphoric expressions, but semantic information has the same effect. 相似文献