首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8401篇
  免费   422篇
  国内免费   5篇
  2023年   93篇
  2022年   120篇
  2021年   154篇
  2020年   204篇
  2019年   245篇
  2018年   384篇
  2017年   377篇
  2016年   368篇
  2015年   250篇
  2014年   267篇
  2013年   1062篇
  2012年   587篇
  2011年   573篇
  2010年   350篇
  2009年   256篇
  2008年   446篇
  2007年   383篇
  2006年   320篇
  2005年   349篇
  2004年   246篇
  2003年   234篇
  2002年   204篇
  2001年   115篇
  2000年   129篇
  1999年   83篇
  1998年   56篇
  1997年   40篇
  1996年   46篇
  1995年   36篇
  1994年   21篇
  1993年   39篇
  1992年   53篇
  1991年   49篇
  1990年   58篇
  1989年   45篇
  1988年   51篇
  1987年   39篇
  1986年   34篇
  1985年   32篇
  1984年   42篇
  1983年   33篇
  1982年   16篇
  1979年   27篇
  1978年   23篇
  1977年   16篇
  1976年   20篇
  1974年   20篇
  1973年   18篇
  1971年   17篇
  1968年   20篇
排序方式: 共有8828条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
73.
74.
75.
Stig Fhanér 《Psychometrika》1966,31(2):267-269
It is shown that certain correlation measures suggested by information theory are simple functions of the determinants of the correlation matrices involved, if multivariate normality is assumed. This illuminates and in a sense strengthens Rozeboom's point in [3], that information theoretical statistics have classical correlational counterparts.  相似文献   
76.
77.
78.
In the Critique of Judgement, Kant, despite his strong condemnation of rhetoric, introduces the figure of hypotyposis at the very moment he sets out to tackle the philosophical problem of presentation as such. This study holds that this choice of the rhetorical term is not fortuitous. Its connotations of vivid illustration, synopsis, and moral grandeur serve Kant in arguing that, on a transcendental level, presentation secures the mind's life, unity, and self-affection. Although of rhetorical origin, hypotyposis is thus shown to link up with a specifically philosophical meaning of the term in the writings of Aristotle.  相似文献   
79.
We used quadratic shapes in several psychophysical shape-from-stereo tasks. The shapes were elegantly represented in a 2-D parameter space by the scale-independentshape index and the scale-dependentcurvedness. Using random-dot stereograms to depict the surfaces, we found that the shape of hyperbolic surfaces is slightly more difficult to recognize than the shape of elliptic surfaces. We found that curvedness (and indirectly, scale) has little or no influence on shape recognition.  相似文献   
80.
When an observer views a moving scene binocularly, both motion parallax and binocular disparity provide depth information. In Experiments lA-1C, we measured sensitivity to surface curvature when these depth cues were available either individually or simultaneously. When the depth cues yielded comparable sensitivity to surface curvature, we found that curvature detection was easier with the cues present simultaneously, rather than individually. For 2 of the 6 subjects, this effect was stronger when the component of frontal translation of the surface was vertical, rather than horizontal. No such anisotropy was found for the 4 other subjects. If a moving object is observed binocularly, the patterns of optic flow are different on the left and right retinae. We have suggested elsewhere (Cornilleau-Pérès & Droulez, in press) that this motion disparity might be used as avisual cue for the perception of a 3-D structure. Our model consisted in deriving binocular disparity from the left and right distributions of vertical velocities, rather than from luminous intensities, as has been done in classical studies on stereoscopic vision. The model led to some predictions concerning the detection of surface curvature from motion disparity in the presence or absence of intensity-based disparity (classically termedbinocular disparity). In a second set of experiments, we attempted to test these predictions, and we failed to validate our theoretical scheme from a physiological point of view.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号