首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   26096篇
  免费   1087篇
  国内免费   16篇
  27199篇
  2020年   290篇
  2019年   397篇
  2018年   541篇
  2017年   573篇
  2016年   612篇
  2015年   456篇
  2014年   489篇
  2013年   2239篇
  2012年   880篇
  2011年   862篇
  2010年   540篇
  2009年   493篇
  2008年   667篇
  2007年   729篇
  2006年   611篇
  2005年   597篇
  2004年   571篇
  2003年   552篇
  2002年   545篇
  2001年   813篇
  2000年   787篇
  1999年   590篇
  1998年   263篇
  1997年   226篇
  1996年   254篇
  1995年   228篇
  1992年   483篇
  1991年   475篇
  1990年   466篇
  1989年   421篇
  1988年   452篇
  1987年   400篇
  1986年   413篇
  1985年   414篇
  1984年   334篇
  1983年   298篇
  1982年   220篇
  1979年   367篇
  1978年   271篇
  1977年   232篇
  1975年   292篇
  1974年   366篇
  1973年   379篇
  1972年   295篇
  1971年   294篇
  1970年   290篇
  1969年   260篇
  1968年   363篇
  1967年   320篇
  1966年   294篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
Brain lateralization research has led to speculation about counseling and guidance implications of left-right brain differences. Serious limitations in these implications are highlighted.  相似文献   
32.
33.
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed.  相似文献   
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
Robertson  Simon 《Synthese》2010,181(1):81-106

What is the relation between what we ought to do, on the one hand, and our epistemic access to the ought-giving facts, on the other? In assessing this, it is common to distinguish ‘objective’ from ‘subjective’ oughts. Very roughly, on the objectivist conception what an agent ought to do is determined by ought-giving facts in such a way that does not depend on the agent’s beliefs about, or epistemic access to, those facts; whereas on the subjectivist conception, what an agent ought to do depends on his beliefs. This paper defends the need for, and explicates, a third category of ‘ought’: ‘warranted oughts’. Section 1 introduces the distinction between objective and subjective ‘oughts’. Sections 2–3 draw attention to some serious problems with each. Section 4 examines, though rejects, a recent attempt to replace subjective ‘oughts’ with objective ‘wide-scope oughts’ operating on belief-action combinations. Section 5 explicates the notion of a warranted ‘ought’ and defends the account against some possible objections. The resulting a picture is one in which an adequate analysis of practical normativity requires both objective and warranted ‘oughts’. Section 6 concludes by responding to a worry about countenancing both.

  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号