首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   24859篇
  免费   257篇
  国内免费   2篇
  25118篇
  2024年   12篇
  2023年   64篇
  2022年   101篇
  2021年   135篇
  2020年   166篇
  2019年   175篇
  2018年   3647篇
  2017年   3005篇
  2016年   2402篇
  2015年   332篇
  2014年   233篇
  2013年   684篇
  2012年   901篇
  2011年   2686篇
  2010年   2699篇
  2009年   1572篇
  2008年   1964篇
  2007年   2376篇
  2006年   234篇
  2005年   342篇
  2004年   277篇
  2003年   212篇
  2002年   140篇
  2001年   78篇
  2000年   114篇
  1999年   60篇
  1998年   44篇
  1997年   36篇
  1996年   20篇
  1995年   16篇
  1994年   20篇
  1993年   17篇
  1992年   19篇
  1991年   23篇
  1990年   26篇
  1989年   21篇
  1988年   18篇
  1987年   19篇
  1986年   14篇
  1985年   15篇
  1982年   9篇
  1981年   13篇
  1979年   11篇
  1973年   11篇
  1972年   12篇
  1971年   16篇
  1969年   10篇
  1968年   17篇
  1966年   12篇
  1965年   14篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 7 毫秒
161.
162.
163.
164.
165.
This work analyzed the effects of unrealistic optimism in the interaction between the emotional valence of future events, the perception of control over these events, and the person with whom one compares oneself. It was hypothesized that, if the person of comparison is judged as very competent, a pessimistic bias should be produced. Likelihood of four different types of events (positive and controllable, positive and uncontrollable, negative and controllable, and negative and uncontrollable) were rated by 133 university students (22 men and 111 women) for themselves, for an average student, for their best friend, and for a bright friend. A pessimistic bias was observed on the relative likelihood of the events when the comparison was made between oneself and a competent and bright friend, when events were perceived as controllable, especially positive ones. Not enough is known, however, to provide meaningful interpretation at present; that must await further data and theoretical development.  相似文献   
166.
Gutiérrez  G.  de Guzmán  I. P.  Martínez  J.  Ojeda-Aciego  M.  Valverde  A. 《Studia Logica》2002,72(1):85-112
The tree-based data structure of -tree for propositional formulas is introduced in an improved and optimised form. The -trees allow a compact representation for negation normal forms as well as for a number of reduction strategies in order to consider only those occurrences of literals which are relevant for the satisfiability of the input formula. These reduction strategies are divided into two subsets (meaning- and satisfiability-preserving transformations) and can be used to decrease the size of a negation normal form A at (at most) quadratic cost. The reduction strategies are aimed at decreasing the number of required branchings and, therefore, these strategies allow to limit the size of the search space for the SAT problem.  相似文献   
167.
Dudek  Józef 《Studia Logica》2002,72(3):363-366
  相似文献   
168.
169.
In the first part of this paper we review evidence suggesting that there exists a mechanism that selects input on the basis of its similarity to the required action. This response-based input selection differs from the more established space- and object-based input selection in that it is not constrained by the structure of the input. Our evidence suggests that the two-choice Stroop effect is caused by this response-based selection mechanism. By contrast, it is known that the flanker effect is determined by the space- and object- based selection mechanisms. We explore whether the conflict resolution of the Stroop and flanker tasks is different as well by embedding these two tasks in a PRP (Psychological Refractory Period) paradigm. We show that the Stroop and the PRP effects are additive whereas the flanker and the PRP effects are underadditive, suggesting that the processes in charge of the conflict resolution in the Stroop and the flanker tasks are indeed different. We discuss possible reasons for this difference, and discuss possible ways in which the response-based mechanism can be implemented in information processing models.  相似文献   
170.
Variability and detection of invariant structure   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Two experiments investigated learning of nonadjacent dependencies by adults and 18–month–olds. Each learner was exposed to three–element strings (e.g., pel–kicey–jic) produced by one of two artificial languages. Both languages contained the same adjacent dependencies, so learners could distinguish the languages only by acquiring dependencies between the first and third elements (the nonadjacent dependencies). The size of the pool from which the middle elements were drawn was systematically varied to investigate whether increasing variability (in the form of decreasing predictability between adjacent elements) would lead to better detection of nonadjacent dependencies. Infants and adults acquired nonadjacent dependencies only when adjacent dependencies were least predictable. The results point to conditions that might lead learners to focus on nonadjacent versus adjacent dependencies and are important for suggesting how learning might be dynamically guided by statistical structure.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号