全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1586篇 |
免费 | 165篇 |
国内免费 | 349篇 |
专业分类
2100篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 19篇 |
2022年 | 37篇 |
2021年 | 66篇 |
2020年 | 72篇 |
2019年 | 47篇 |
2018年 | 79篇 |
2017年 | 71篇 |
2016年 | 75篇 |
2015年 | 54篇 |
2014年 | 63篇 |
2013年 | 143篇 |
2012年 | 95篇 |
2011年 | 120篇 |
2010年 | 113篇 |
2009年 | 110篇 |
2008年 | 126篇 |
2007年 | 156篇 |
2006年 | 155篇 |
2005年 | 111篇 |
2004年 | 77篇 |
2003年 | 57篇 |
2002年 | 42篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 15篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有2100条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
谊研宛采用工作压力源量表、压力反应量表以及积极心理资本问卷对天津市某企业230名员工进行了调查。分析了心理资本与工作压力源和压力反应的关系及其作用机制。结果发现:(1)工作压力源对压力反应有显著的正向预洲作用;(2)心理资本对压力反应有显著的负向预测作用;(3)心理资本在工作压力源与压力反应之间起部分中介作用而不是调节作用,其中中介效应占总效应的比例为16%;(4)心理资本在各种工作压力源与压力反应问均起到中介作用,中介效应和直接效应的比值介于17.4%-26.1%之间。 相似文献
102.
Fanghong Dong Fenglin Cao Peixia Cheng Naixue Cui Yang Li 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2013,54(5):415-422
Multiple types of victimization or poly‐victimization (PV) can occur simultaneously during childhood, resulting in outcomes that are detrimental and difficult to reverse. Very few studies have focused on PV in Chinese adolescents. The present study is based on information collected from a stratified cluster sample of 1,561 females and 1,594 males aged 12–18 years living in Shandong Province, China. A self‐administered questionnaire was used to assess background information, the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire was used to measure the extent of victimization and PV, the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders was used to measure anxiety levels, and a Depression Self‐rating Scale for Children was used to ascertain depression. A majority of the study sample (66.2%) had experienced at least one form of victimization over the past year. Prevalence of PV was 16.9%, and was significantly higher among males (21.1%) than females (12.5%). Younger ages, one‐child families, and lower socioeconomic status were positively associated with PV. Logistic regression analysis indicated that smoking, alcohol consumption, exposure to pornography, and anxiety‐ and depression‐like symptoms were risk factors of PV. Results of this study highlight the need for further exploration of factors related to the PV of Chinese adolescents. 相似文献
103.
Zing Yang Kuo 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2013,174(2):195-209
An attempt was made to investigate the hypothesis that type of abstract reasoning impairment was related to age. Forty brain damaged patients were divided into four age groups (30–39, 40–49, 50–59, and 60+) and evaluated by a concept identification task which yields data as to whether impairment is qualitative or quantitative. The results indicated that the greatest amount of qualitative type impairment occurred among the older brain damaged patients when compared with the other subjects. 相似文献
104.
Eunsuk Yang 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》2013,42(5):767-782
This paper first shows that some versions of the logic R of Relevance do not satisfy the relevance principle introduced by Anderson and Belnap, the principle of which is generally accepted as the principle for relevance. After considering several possible (but defective) improvements of the relevance principle, this paper presents a new relevance principle for (three versions of) R, and explains why this principle is better than the original and others. 相似文献
105.
Xufeng Liu Yang Liao Luping Zhou Gang Sun Min Li Lun Zhao 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2013,13(3):491-500
The aim of the present study was to investigate the time course of the positive advantage in the expression classification of faces by recording event-related potentials (ERPs). Although neutral faces were classified more quickly than either happy or sad faces, a significant positive classification advantage (PCA)—that is, faster classification for happy than for sad faces—was found. For ERP data, as compared with sad faces, happy faces elicited a smaller N170 and a larger posterior N2 component. The P3 was modulated by facial expressions with higher amplitudes and shorter latencies for both happy and neutral stimuli than for sad stimuli, and the reaction times were significantly correlated with the amplitude and latency of the P3. Overall, these data showed robust PCA in expression classification, starting when the stimulus has been recognized as a face revealed by the N170 component. 相似文献
106.
先秦时期管理心理思想研究 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
本文首次对我国先秦时期丰富的管理心理思想予以概论,涉及的方面有人性假设、管理策略、管理者的人格等。同时兼与西方现代有关管理心理思想进行比较。 相似文献
107.
语义启动模型以及启动范围 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
启动效应一般分为两种——知觉启动和语义启动(概念启动)。语义启动的范式逐渐成为研究语义知识在大脑中是如何存储的主要研究手段。该文针对语义启动发生的3种机制:激活扩散模型,复合线索模型和分布式记忆模型进行了简要的评述,并且重点介绍了分布式记忆模型。同时对启动效应可以激活语义知识的范围作了一个简要的概括。 相似文献
108.
The motion of objects that are both translating and rotating can be decomposed into an infinite number of translational and rotational combinations. How, then, do such stimuli routinely elicit specific percepts and behavioral responses that are usually appropriate? A possible answer is that motion percepts are fully determined by the probability distributions of all the possible correspondences and differences in the stimulus sequence. To test the merits of this conceptual framework, we investigated the perceived motion elicited by a line that is both translating and rotating behind an aperture. When stimuli are presented such that a particular sequence of appearance and disappearance occurs at the aperture boundary, subjects report that the line is rotating only; furthermore, the perceived centers of rotation appear to describe a cycloidal trajectory, even when one aperture shape is replaced by another. These and other perceptual effects elicited by translating and rotating stimuli are all accurately predicted by the probability distribution of the possible sources of the physical movements, supporting the conclusion that motion perception is indeed generated by a wholly probabilistic strategy. 相似文献
109.
Some theories of associative learning imply that time plays a fundamental role in the acquisition process. Consistent with these theories, this paper presents evidence that the time from the onset of a conditioned stimulus (CS) until presentation of the unconditioned stimulus (US) is learned very rapidly at the start of training. We report two autoshaping studies and a study on aversive conditioning in goldfish in which we examine timing at the start of conditioning. We also review data from a number of other conditioning preparations, including fear-potentiated startle, appetitive conditioning in rats, and eyeblink conditioning in rabbits, that report conditioned response (CR) timing early in training. Acquisition speed and the very first expressions of conditioned responding often show sensitivity to the time of US presentation. In instances where temporal control is slowly expressed, it is likely due to performance factors, not to slow learning about time. In fact, the learning about time may be a necessary condition for associative learning. 相似文献
110.
为了探讨哪一类别商品的平面广告更适合于采用幽默的诉求方式,研究随机选取160大学生为被试,以16则改编平面广告为实验材料,以广告类型(幽默,非幽默)、商品类别为自变量,以广告评价、广告顺序为协变量,以商品的印象程度、喜爱程度、购买意向为因变量,进行平衡设计实验。结果表明,幽默广告具有较好广告效果,可以提高受众对大宗工具、小奖赏和小工具型商品的记忆;幽默平面广告更适用于低风险、功能性商品,不适用于高风险、享乐性商品。 相似文献