首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   526篇
  免费   46篇
  国内免费   148篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   13篇
  2021年   14篇
  2020年   18篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   27篇
  2016年   25篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   42篇
  2012年   20篇
  2011年   53篇
  2010年   31篇
  2009年   36篇
  2008年   41篇
  2007年   46篇
  2006年   64篇
  2005年   40篇
  2004年   24篇
  2003年   17篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   14篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   17篇
  1996年   11篇
  1995年   14篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   2篇
  1979年   3篇
排序方式: 共有720条查询结果,搜索用时 328 毫秒
631.
中国远程学习者学习风格理论模型与验证   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
朱祖林  安哲锋  陈丽 《心理科学》2008,31(6):1521-1523
为帮助中国远程学习者了解自身的学习风格,选择相应的学习策略,进而提高远程学习质量,根据学习风格的信息加工理论、Kolb的经验学习理论以及荣格的人格特征类型理论,从生理、社会、心理三个维度,构建了我国远程学习者学习风格的理论模型,通过编制量表和实际测量,对该模型进行验证分析和修订完善.结果表明,最初提出的理论模型假设得到验证,修订后的量表具有良好的效度和信度,可以用来测量我国远程学习者的学习风格.  相似文献   
632.
Different thinking styles in Westerners and Chinese (analytic vs. holistic) lead to disparities between the two cultures not only in perception and attention but also in high‐level social cognition such as self‐representation. Most Western philosophers discussed the self by focusing on personal self‐identity, whereas Chinese philosophers emphasized the relation between the self and others. Dissimilar philosophical thinking of the self is associated with distinct cognitive styles of self‐representation (i.e., the independent self in Westerners and the interdependent self in Chinese). Recent brain imaging studies found that Westerners employed the medial prefrontal cortex to represent only the individual self, whereas Chinese utilized the same brain area to represent both the self and close others, providing neural basis of cultural differences in self‐representation. We suggest that the cultural differences in thinking styles between Westerners and Chinese influence both psychological and neural structure of self‐representation.  相似文献   
633.
国外社会心理学领域中主观幸福感的研究进展   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
社会心理学领域扩展与深化了现代主观幸福感的研究。本文介绍了国外社会心理学领域中有关主观幸福感研究的5个方面的新进展:适应与主观幸福感、记忆与主观幸福感、消极因素与主观幸福感、友好关系与主观幸福感、文化与主观幸福感,勾画出了幸福感的完整风貌,增进了心理学对主观幸福感的理解。  相似文献   
634.
郭力平  郝俊  朱文佳 《心理科学》2012,35(4):882-888
本研究采用表征理解任务和规则学习任务,通过比较真人互动、视频拟互动和视频非互动三种条件下年幼儿童的学习效果,考察拟互动在2-5岁儿童视频学习中的作用。结果表明,2岁儿童的视频学习存在视频缺陷现象,2.5岁到5岁儿童的拟社会互动视频学习效果明显,优于非互动视频学习,与真人互动学习相当。2.5岁至4.5岁是拟互动视频学习的最佳年龄。  相似文献   
635.
Liu  Weishu  Zhu  Junwen  Zuo  Chao  Wang  Haiyan 《Science and engineering ethics》2019,25(2):647-649
Science and Engineering Ethics - By following a recently published paper entitled “The effect of publishing a highly cited paper on a journal’s impact factor: a case study of the Review...  相似文献   
636.
2007年11月21日,贵州省道教第一次代表会议在贵阳举行.贵州省人大常委会副主任杨序颐、政协副主席唐世礼出席会议并讲话.会议审议并通过了<贵州省道教协会章程>,选举产生了贵州省道教协会第一届理事会和领导班子,张崇新当选为省道教协会会长,徐达双、唐至宗、邓信德、令狐昌乾等4人当选为副会长.  相似文献   
637.
Kahneman & Tversky (1982 ) demonstrated that actions are regretted more than inactions. It was conjectured that (a) when action shared the same closeness as inaction but was neither to approach nor to retreat the desired outcome, the action was seen as futile so that it would evoke stronger reactions of regret; and (b) closeness moderated the effect of action such that the effect was stronger when the acting target was closer to a good outcome, but weaker when the acting target was further away from a good outcome. A total of 5 regret‐triggering problems were presented to 150 undergraduates who rated actors' intensity of regret. The findings support that closeness is the moderator of the increased regret effect.  相似文献   
638.
Participants were shown rapid sequences of three letters, flanked by digits, each rotated 0 degree, 90 degrees, 180 degrees, or 270 degrees clockwise from upright. In Experiment 1, the participants tried to report the letter that matched the orientation of an arrow, presented either before (before task) or after (after task) the sequence. A third task (total task) required them to report all of the letters. Accuracy for individual letters was significantly better in the total task than in the before task, and better in the before task than in the after task, suggesting particular difficulty in binding orientation to identity. In Experiment 2, the participants were given letter probes and were asked to indicate the orientation of the probed letter. Although report was above chance, there were frequent illusory conjunctions. Since perception of orientation must depend on prior establishment of identity, our results suggest that orientation and identity may become unbound during processing and are held in parallel storage systems.  相似文献   
639.
去年11月15日,中国科学院自然科学史所研究员宋正海通过电子邮件发出倡议信,题目是《不要让"伪科学"一词成为灭亡传统文化的借口》,  相似文献   
640.
The Predominantly Inattentive (PI) and Combined (CB) subtypes of AD/HD differ in cognitive tempo, age of onset, gender ratio, and comorbidity, yet a differentiating endophenotype has not been identified. The aim of this study was to test rigorously diagnosed PI, CB, and typical children on measures selected for their potential to reveal hypothesized differences between the subtypes in specific neurocognitive systems (anterior vs. posterior attentional systems) and processes (arousal vs. activation). Thirty-four CB and 26 PI children meeting full DSM-IV criteria for subtype both in school and at home, without confounding reading disability or emotional disorder, were enrolled along with 20 typically developing children. Neurocognitive functions measured included attention, inhibitory control, working memory, learning, and executive functions. Tasks included the Stroop, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Continuous Performance Test (CPT). Buschke Selective Reminding Test, ad the Tower of London (TOL), as well as instruments developed by Posner and Sternberg, and tasks assessing the impact on reaction time of [corrected] varying preparatory intervals and stimulus/response complexity. After co-varying for IQ, subtypes differed primarily on measures of impulsivity during tests of vigilance (CPT) and executive function (TOL), with the CB group showing greater impulsivity than both other groups. In addition, the PI group showed worse performance than CB and control groups on the WISC-III Processing Speed Index. Whether analyzed with or without an IQ co-variate, there was no support in the data for hypothesized differences between subtypes in functioning of the anterior vs. posterior attentional systems, nor in involvement of arousal vs. activation processes. The results indicate that the PI and CB subtypes are best differentiated by ratings, observations and tests of cognitive tempo and behavioral impulsivity. Neuropsychological methods have yet to identify critical neuropsychological [corrected] substrates of these differences.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号