首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   623篇
  免费   82篇
  国内免费   274篇
  2023年   12篇
  2022年   22篇
  2021年   25篇
  2020年   23篇
  2019年   35篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   33篇
  2016年   24篇
  2015年   37篇
  2014年   32篇
  2013年   55篇
  2012年   40篇
  2011年   56篇
  2010年   50篇
  2009年   42篇
  2008年   55篇
  2007年   59篇
  2006年   74篇
  2005年   64篇
  2004年   41篇
  2003年   30篇
  2002年   25篇
  2001年   17篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   1篇
  1970年   1篇
排序方式: 共有979条查询结果,搜索用时 109 毫秒
81.
美国的宗教状况一直是宗教学界极为关注的问题。而世界上颇多著名的调查机构都对此问题有所涉及。本文就结合盖洛普咨询公司(The Gallup Organization)、美国哈里斯互动调研公司(Harris Interactive Inc.)、加拿大安格斯列特策略(Angus Reid Strategies)、法国益普索市场研究集团公共事务部(Ipsos Public Affairs)的调查数据.对美国的宗教信仰状况、宗教在日常生活中的地位以及宗教与社会的关系进行一初步的分析。  相似文献   
82.
我国中学生的一般能力因素结构探讨   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
方俐洛  韩聪  凌文辁 《心理学报》1998,31(3):306-312
该研究在对一般能力倾向测验的因素分析基础上,提出了三因素能力结构模型,揭示了中国中学生有一般能力包括。数理能力,知觉能力,心理运动能力三个因素,共8种性向。  相似文献   
83.
社会认知中印象形成的启动效应研究:频因,近因效应   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
钟毅平  杨治良 《心理科学》1998,21(5):425-428
本文以社会认知为出发点,考察印象形成的启动效应,它采用性质不同的成语作为实验材料,并操纵启动刺激的结构,系统探讨了印象形成的启动效应中原因与近因效应,结果发现:当呈现表示同一品质但性质相反的成语时,如果启动刺激和目标刺激之间不延迟,则近因在启动效应中起作用,但在延迟情况下,频因在启动效应中占优势;同时随着延迟的出现,启动效应明显变小。实验结果支持启动效应的突触模式说;  相似文献   
84.
初中一,二年级学生的亲子冲突   总被引:25,自引:1,他引:24  
方晓义  董奇 《心理科学》1998,21(2):122-125
本研究选取北京市两所普通中学192名初一和初二学生为被试,通过学生对8个方面亲子冲突的评价,探讨初中生亲子冲突的特点。研究发现,(1)初中生早期的亲子冲突处于较低水平;(2)与母亲的冲突多于与父亲的冲突,女生尤其如此;(3)多数亲子冲突随年级而增加;(4)在多数冲突上,男生与父母的冲突多于女生;(5)亲子冲突最多的三个方面为日常生活安排、学业和家务,而冲突最少的为隐私。  相似文献   
85.
86.
87.
Rural to urban migration has become a prominent phenomenon in China. In linking migration to children’s school trajectories, this mixed-methods study explored a range of cultural and contextual factors that contribute to Chinese children’s school wellbeing in the migratory process. The key variables included acculturative attitudes, economic stress, and relationships with family, teachers and peers. The study drew upon survey data from a sample of 301 Chinese migrant students (4th–9th grade) together with in-depth interviews involving ten selected students focusing on how migrant children’s school happiness and productivity are influenced by acculturation, economic hardship, and social relationships. The quantitative analysis indicated the critical role of integrative states in promoting school satisfaction and achievement. Migrant children with better social relations scored higher levels of hope, which in turn contributed to better school outcomes. Contrary to previous findings, economic stress did not hinder school outcomes for these Chinese migrant children, but appeared to act as a positive motivational factor for pursing academic success. The qualitative analysis echoed the quantitative findings, and provided further explanations for the complexity and particularity of these phenomena.  相似文献   
88.
This paper tested the impact of culture on the basic structure of affect. It examined positive affect and negative affect at three levels of activation in the U.S. and China. It used a well-suited tool, the bifactor model, to separate the common variance shared by all types of affect from the variance unique to each. The findings indicate that the structure of affect is different cross-culturally. In the U.S., the most fundamental dimension is defined by moderate activation of positive affect and negative affect, which is the largely bipolar dimension of pleasure-displeasure, whereas in China, it is defined by moderate activation of positive affect only, which is pleasure. In both cultures, negative affect across levels of activation forms another important dimension. Beyond these basic dimensions, secondary dimensions are also identified in both cultures: high activation positive affect, low activation positive affect, and low activation negative affect. They form relatively weak unique factors, independent of the two basic dimensions, suggesting that they are largely mixtures of the basic dimensions.  相似文献   
89.
Although several studies have compared the representation of fractions and decimals, no study has investigated whether fractions and decimals, as two types of rational numbers, share a common representation of magnitude. The current study aimed to answer the question of whether fractions and decimals share a common representation of magnitude and whether the answer is influenced by task paradigms. We included two different number pairs, which were presented sequentially: fraction–decimal mixed pairs and decimal–fraction mixed pairs in all four experiments. Results showed that when the mixed pairs were very close numerically with the distance 0.1 or 0.3, there was a significant distance effect in the comparison task but not in the matching task. However, when the mixed pairs were further apart numerically with the distance 0.3 or 1.3, the distance effect appeared in the matching task regardless of the specific stimuli. We conclude that magnitudes of fractions and decimals can be represented in a common manner, but how they are represented is dependent on the given task. Fractions and decimals could be translated into a common representation of magnitude in the numerical comparison task. In the numerical matching task, fractions and decimals also shared a common representation. However, both of them were represented coarsely, leading to a weak distance effect. Specifically, fractions and decimals produced a significant distance effect only when the numerical distance was larger.  相似文献   
90.
The present study aimed to examine the child‐rearing practices in Chinese families in Beijing and Hong Kong. The sample consisted of 89 mothers in Beijing and 45 mothers in Hong Kong. The mothers were instructed to respond to the Child‐rearing Practice Report (CRPR) in Q‐sort format. Some of the item clusters were combined to produce the authoritarian and authoritative disciplinary styles. The results indicated that mothers in Hong Kong were more likely to adopt an authoritarian child‐rearing pattern than mothers in Beijing; however, the two groups did not differ in authoritative child‐rearing style. The results also showed that mothers in Hong Kong controlled their children more than their counterparts in Beijing, and they were less inclined to show affection towards their children; mothers in Beijing emphasized their children's achievement much more than their Hong Kong counterparts. The findings suggest that Chinese parental disciplinary styles may be quite different in various regions of Chinese societies. Such variance across different geopolitical locations within the same cultural background has been ignored in past cross‐cultural research.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号