全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1240篇 |
免费 | 46篇 |
专业分类
1286篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 13篇 |
2021年 | 21篇 |
2020年 | 36篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 37篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 55篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 120篇 |
2012年 | 38篇 |
2011年 | 48篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 43篇 |
2007年 | 40篇 |
2006年 | 37篇 |
2005年 | 31篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 22篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 24篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1286条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
191.
This article presents an assessment of the relative influences of time spent participating in organized sports and informal sports during childhood with respect to the development of general creativity. In this study, 99 upper-division undergraduate and graduate students completed a comprehensive childhood leisure activities questionnaire and the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults. According to the results of the joint generalized least-squares regression analysis, hours spent in organized sport settings was negatively related to creativity as an adult; time spent in unstructured sport settings was found to be positively related to adult creativity. The findings also point to the importance of balancing participation across organized and unstructured settings. The most creative individuals in the sample were those who spent roughly half of their sport participation time in each setting, as opposed to individuals with below-average creativity, who spent upwards of 3/4 of their sport participation time in organized settings. Therefore, fostering creative development through sport may not require a dramatic reorientation from current youth sport development models, but only a shift toward a more balanced distribution of time spent playing in both organized and unstructured settings. Future experiments are needed to test this relationship. 相似文献
192.
Luís Oliveira Leonard Green Joel Myerson 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2014,102(2):151-161
193.
Ana V. Nikčević Gabriele Caselli Deborah Green Marcantonio M. Spada 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2014,32(4):248-256
Both perceived stress and negative recurrent thinking (rumination and worry) have been associated with depressive symptoms. However, no research to date has investigated the association between perceived stress and negative recurrent thinking. In the present study we aimed to explore whether perceived stress and negative recurrent thinking are associated and whether negative recurrent thinking moderates the relationship between perceived stress and depressive symptoms. A convenience sample of 273 undergraduate students completed the Perceived Stress Scale, the Ruminative Responses Scale-10, the Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale 2 weeks prior to sitting mid-year examinations. Correlation analyses showed that perceived stress, rumination and worry were positively and significantly associated with depressive symptoms and that perceived stress was positively and significantly associated with rumination and worry. A moderation analysis confirmed that negative recurrent thinking moderated the relationship between perceived stress and depressive symptoms. The implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
194.
Adele Green 《Brain and cognition》1986,5(4):477-497
Several methodological factors associated with the concurrent activities (finger-tapping) paradigm were considered in a cross-sectional study investigating cerebral patterns of asymmetry in three groups of English-speaking non-Hispanic dextral males at three levels of second language (Spanish) acquisition and one control group of monolinguals. Results revealed the fluent bilinguals to be bilateral and significantly different from other groups for native language tasks in English. Moreover, a priori contrasts indicate that greater right- than left-hand disruption in concurrent tapping may be typical of monolinguals, but can be influenced by other factors. Monolingual reliability test-retest correlations were .77 and .47. 相似文献
195.
Frank H. Walkey Dianne E. Green Iain A. McCormick 《Personality and individual differences》1986,7(6):903-905
Evidence for the existence of the assumed three-subscale structure of the Eysenck Personality Inventory has been persistent but tenuous. It was suggested that a substantial reason for this may be the discrepancy between the numbers of items in the Lie (L) scale (9 items) and those in the other two scales [Neuroticism (N) and Extraversion (E)] which have 24 items in each. A two-step confirmatory procedure to circumvent this problem was proposed, involving a two-factor analysis of the Neuroticism and Extraversion items, followed by a three-factor analysis of equal numbers of N, E and L scale items. An analysis of the responses of 386 undergraduates demonstrating the proposed procedure revealed in the first step, quite unequivocally, the presence of Neuroticism and Extraversion factors, and in the second step, a third factor clearly congruent with the L scale. 相似文献
196.
197.
The authors used a representative national sample (N = 777) to test the evolutionary hypothesis that men would be more bothered by sexual infidelity and women by emotional infidelity, the Jealousy as a Specific Innate Module (JSIM) effect. Our alternative conceptualization of jealousy suggests that there are distinct emotional components of jealousy that did not evolve differently by gender. The authors looked for effects of age, socioeconomic status (SES), and type of measure (continuous or dichotomous) on jealousy. The authors did not find age or SES effects. Forced-choice items provided support for our alternative view; both genders showed more anger and blame over sexual infidelity but more hurt feelings over emotional infidelity. Continuous measures indicated more emotional response to sexual than emotional infidelity among both genders. 相似文献
198.
The authors evaluated whether self-reported trait anhedonia in schizophrenia reflects faulty memory, such that patients are capable of experiencing pleasure while engaged in enjoyable activities but underestimate their pleasure in recalling these experiences. Thirty schizophrenia patients and 31 nonpatient control participants rated their emotional responses to pleasant and neutral foods and film clips and completed a surprise recall task for their emotions after a 4-hr delay. Despite reporting elevated trait anhedonia, patients did not significantly differ from control participants in immediate pleasant emotional responses to the stimuli or in delayed recall for these experiences. In-the-moment pleasure and short-term retention for emotional experiences thus appear to be relatively intact in schizophrenia. Alternative explanations for the hedonic deficit in this disorder are discussed. 相似文献
199.
Green RE Christensen B Melo B Monette G Bayley M Hebert D Inness E Mcilroy W 《Brain and cognition》2006,60(2):199-201
The status of neurorehabilitation for traumatic brain injury (TBI) is under active debate because of a dearth of research findings demonstrating effectiveness. This may be due, in part, to limitations in our understanding of basic mechanisms of cognitive and motor recovery,including those that might impede recovery. In this regard, we examined whether overall recovery following TBI might be undermined by competition between cognitive and motor functions for finite neural resources during recovery. In this preliminary study, 21 moderately and severely impaired patients were administered cognitive and motor assessments at 1, 4, and 12 months post-TBI, and recovery of cognitive and motor functions was measured using regression residuals. Negative correlations between recovery of cognitive versus motor functions were used as the index of competition. We found suggestive evidence that there may indeed be a trade-off between the recovery of cognitive and motor functions after TBI. Implications for rehabilitation are discussed. 相似文献
200.
Steven J. Danish Susanna S. Owens Scott L. Green John P Brunelle 《Journal of Applied Sport Psychology》2013,25(1):154-167
Abstract A general dircwion for understanding the process and impact of disengagement and uansition on individual athletes and teams is presented. Disengagement is presented as a process within athlete and team development. The disengagement of athletes from a team and the impact on the team ue examined. Two models of change are described as is a model of group development. Examples of a team in transition are provided and suggestions for sports psychologists' work with athletes and teams in transition an offered. The focus of these suggestions is on work with teams. 相似文献