首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   523篇
  免费   70篇
  国内免费   104篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   12篇
  2022年   36篇
  2021年   22篇
  2020年   37篇
  2019年   23篇
  2018年   32篇
  2017年   29篇
  2016年   28篇
  2015年   23篇
  2014年   30篇
  2013年   55篇
  2012年   27篇
  2011年   45篇
  2010年   27篇
  2009年   20篇
  2008年   29篇
  2007年   37篇
  2006年   34篇
  2005年   34篇
  2004年   21篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   9篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   5篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
排序方式: 共有697条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
681.
Findings on the effect of social status on corruption perception are mixed. To make sense of the mixed results, three studies were conducted to examine whether meritocracy moderates this effect. In Study 1, we measured all variables using a questionnaire-based correlational design. In the preregistered Study 2, we adopted an experimental design in which we manipulated social status using a false feedback paradigm and measured corruption perception and meritocracy. In Study 3, we manipulated meritocracy using a scrambled sentence task and measured its effect on the relationship between social status and corruption perception. The results consistently demonstrated that meritocracy could moderate the influence of social status on corruption perception. Specifically, social status was negatively associated with corruption perception when meritocracy was weak, whereas the association was not significant when meritocracy was strong. Our results suggest that meritocracy critically affects acknowledgment of the pervasiveness of social injustice and even deters the disadvantaged from fighting against corruption.  相似文献   
682.
劝善书产生于北宋时期,是一种宣传伦理道德、劝诫世人行善去恶并建立道德规范的指导书.文昌信仰是由产生于远古的星宿信仰、晋朝武将张恶子信仰等合流的一种民间信仰.文昌劝善书是以文昌信仰为基础所造做的劝善书,成为明清时期劝善书的主流.本文主要以一些明清时期重要文昌劝善书为例,探讨文昌神谕性的训诫及其对社会的影响.  相似文献   
683.
684.
685.
Abstract

The co-occurrence of behaviors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) with behaviors of anxiety or behaviors of depression is the norm, but little is known on how the co-occurrence accounted for youths’ peer relations. The authors report results on difficult peer relations in relation to behaviors of ADHD, co-occurring behaviors of depression, and behaviors of anxiety from three studies on 862 youths in China and in the United States. Study 1 included 313 ethnically and socioeconomically diverse American youths; Study 2 included 250 youths who were adopted out of Chinese orphanages by American parents; and Study 3 included 299 youths from Beijing, China. Data on difficult peer relations and behaviors of ADHD, depression, and anxiety were collected with the third edition of Behavior Assessment System for Children-Self Report of Personality. In all three studies, each type of problems alone significantly predicted difficult peer relations, but behaviors of ADHD were not significant when co-occurring behaviors of depression or co-occurring behaviors of anxiety were considered. Despite that the youths in our study had different cultural and personal backgrounds, there was no evidence that behaviors of ADHD were detrimental to youths’ peer relations when behaviors of depression or anxiety were considered. Implications for intervention were discussed.  相似文献   
686.
信息侵权的伦理规约   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
信息权利已经成为当代公民最基本的权利之一,但其实现状况却不尽如人意。信息侵权行为大量涌现,究其原因,信息伦理观念的紊乱、信息道德评价的困难、信息伦理规范的失效与信息伦理教育弱化等因素,都加剧了公民信息权利的危机。为此,从伦理层面上,应该通过营造健康的舆论导向、对把关人角色进行重塑、培育公民的责任感、采取他律与自律相结合、加强对公民的信息伦理教育等伦理规约手段,有效遏制信息侵权行为的泛滥。  相似文献   
687.
688.
689.
Burgeoning evidence suggests that when children observe data, they use knowledge of the demonstrator's intent to augment learning. We propose that the effects of social learning may go beyond cases where children observe data, to cases where they receive no new information at all. We present a model of how simply asking a question a second time may lead to belief revision, when the questioner is expected to know the correct answer. We provide an analysis of the CHILDES corpus to show that these neutral follow-up questions are used in parent–child conversations. We then present three experiments investigating 4- and 5-year-old children's reactions to neutral follow-up questions posed by ignorant or knowledgeable questioners. Children were more likely to change their answers in response to a neutral follow-up question from a knowledgeable questioner than an ignorant one. We discuss the implications of these results in the context of common practices in legal, educational, and experimental psychological settings.  相似文献   
690.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号