首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   222篇
  免费   13篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   7篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   3篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   18篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   7篇
  2010年   13篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   12篇
  2007年   11篇
  2006年   13篇
  2005年   9篇
  2004年   4篇
  2003年   6篇
  2002年   8篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   1篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1970年   1篇
  1969年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
排序方式: 共有235条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Predictions by social identity theory (SIT) and relative deprivation theory (RDT) concerning preferences for strategies to cope with a negative in-group status position were tested. The focus of the present research was a comparison of the theories regarding their differential patterns of prediction. For this purpose, a natural sample within a specific historical situation was investigated: East Germans after the German unification. First, the predictive power of SIT and RDT variables was tested separately. In a second step, a possible integration of the theories was addressed. Combining the SIT variables and RDT variables led to an integrated model indicating a differential pattern of prediction for intergroup strategies. The RDT components explained the collective responses, whereas SIT constructs were related to individual strategies.  相似文献   
62.
63.
Certain classes of verbal behavior—content summary statements (Sc); affective summary statements (Sa); continuity (C), justification (J), and well-wishing (W) statements; and statements of positive affect (P)—were hypothesized to be constituents of interpersonal endings. These statements are shown to increase during the terminal phase of a social encounter as a function of whether the conversation is between friends or strangers and whether it is structured or unstructured. A hypothesis concerning the sequence in which these five classes of statements will occur during the ending of an interaction, namely, [Sc] → [J] → [SaP] → [C] → [W], is tested and confirmed.  相似文献   
64.
Measuring Daily Events and Experiences: Decisions for the Researcher   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
There has been a burgeoning interest in studying daily events and experiences. This article discusses a variety of methodologic challenges that face daily event and experience researchers. The issues discussed include techniques for measuring events, the development of event checklists, sampling event content, specifying event appraisals, event validation procedures, and the creation of summary measures derived from event checklists. Procedural issues discussed include determining the number of observations and persons needed for daily event studies, the evaluation of response, attrition, and missing item bias, and problems linking event reports over time.  相似文献   
65.
66.
The influence of feedback representation on prediction is examined in a single cue probability learning paradigm. Two types of feedback representation are examined: deviation representation, in which the feedback is the magnitude, or even just the sign, of the prediction error, and standard representation, in which the feedback is the outcome itself. It is found that when the predictor is represented visually (rather than numerically), and when the outcome scale is unknown, deviation representation results in higher prediction extremity than standard representation. In addition, deviation representation results in higher prediction consistency than standard representation. These findings are explained as resulting from more reliance on the representativeness heuristic in the deviation representation conditions.  相似文献   
67.
68.
English spelling is highly inconsistent in terms of simple sound-to-spelling correspondences but is more consistent when context is taken into account. For example, the choice between ch and tch is determined by the preceding vowel (coach, roach vs. catch, hatch). We investigated children's sensitivity to vowel context when spelling consonants in monosyllabic nonwords. Second graders (7-year-olds) tended to use vowel context correctly when spelling word-final consonants (codas). This use of context was progressively stronger for third and fifth graders as well as for college students. The increase is not due to differences in vocabulary because the contextual patterns are similar in reading materials targeted at all four age groups. Vowel letters (graphotactics) had a stronger influence than did vowel pronunciation. Children also used vowel context when spelling word-initial consonants (onsets); this effect was as strong for second graders as for adults. Thus, novice spellers take advantage of graphotactic information.  相似文献   
69.
In English and some other languages, spelling problems that arise at a phonological level can sometimes be solved through consideration of morphology. For example, children could infer that tuned should contain an n and that fighting should contain a t because their stems include these letters. Children could thus avoid misspellings that might otherwise occur, such as "tud" and "fiding." We used a spelling-level match design to examine the extent to which children with dyslexia and younger typical children use morphology in this way. Both groups of children benefited from morphology to some extent, but not as much as they could have given their knowledge of the stems. Our results suggest that the spellings produced by older children with dyslexia are similar to those of younger normal children in their morphological characteristics, as well as in other ways.  相似文献   
70.
According to construal level theory (N. Liberman, Y. Trope, & E. Stephan, in press; Y. Trope & N. Liberman, 2003), people use a more abstract, high construal level when judging, perceiving, and predicting more psychologically distal targets, and they judge more abstract targets as being more psychologically distal. The present research demonstrated that associations between more distance and higher level of construal also exist on a pure conceptual level. Eight experiments used the Implicit Association Test (IAT; A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. K. Schwartz, 1998) to demonstrate an association between words related to construal level (low vs. high) and words related to four dimensions of distance (proximal vs. distal): temporal distance, spatial distance, social distance, and hypotheticality. In addition to demonstrating an association between level of construal and psychological distance, these findings also corroborate the assumption that all 4 dimensions of psychological distance are related to level of construal in a similar way and support the notion that they all are forms of psychological distance.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号