首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   862篇
  免费   96篇
  国内免费   270篇
  2023年   15篇
  2022年   27篇
  2021年   23篇
  2020年   44篇
  2019年   37篇
  2018年   37篇
  2017年   38篇
  2016年   35篇
  2015年   30篇
  2014年   34篇
  2013年   70篇
  2012年   49篇
  2011年   77篇
  2010年   53篇
  2009年   65篇
  2008年   66篇
  2007年   112篇
  2006年   83篇
  2005年   70篇
  2004年   30篇
  2003年   19篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   21篇
  1999年   21篇
  1998年   12篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   8篇
  1994年   13篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   10篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   4篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   5篇
  1960年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1228条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
851.
Ⅱ型糖尿病患者的行为特征研究   总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13  
使用修订行为特征问卷调查了112名Ⅱ型糖尿病患者和147名健康对照组。结果表明: 1.男性患者比健康人更加抑郁;2.女性患者比健康人更加焦虑、抑郁和愤怒;3.不论是男性 还是女性,此问卷可显著地区分这两组,越抑郁的人,越不焦虑的人,越缺少合理化这种心理 防御机制的人,越可能是糖尿病患者。结合其它的研究结果,认为存在着Ⅱ型糖尿病易感性 行为特征,主要表现为:高抑郁;低焦虑;对应激的唤醒水平低;不善于使用心理防御机制来 保护和伪装自己;寻找一些有趣的事情:回避痛苦事件;不善于延迟的满足,要求马上得到满 足;注意易分散等。  相似文献   
852.
本文是用纵向法,对一个儿童(四周岁至五周岁),掌握数概念的过程,进行追踪研究,提出的报告。 研究结果表明:在有效的训练和充分的练习条件下,被试掌握最初数概念的过程,进入关键性的一年。在这过程中,读写数字、用数字作为符号进行运算,具有特别突出的地位。认读顺序数字表,转化为口头计数能力的发展,是以尝试错误、用手指自我暗示、顿悟为中介。按各种数序进行计数,是口头计数能力发展的主要内容。按物计数能力的发展,明显地比口头计数能力的发展,处于领先的地位。按物计数能力发展的程序是:逐个计数,按群计数,成组计数。认读各种数字表,使被试在实际上,理解和领会数词的基本类型、组合类型及其内在联系。书写数字就是把计数能力的发展,以物质的形式“固定”下来。读写数字使儿童在实际上,掌握和运用数词的“构词法”,促进计数能力的发展,是提高儿童数概念水平的基本方面。被试在掌握数概念的过程中,运算能力发展的程序是:逐个加减,按群加减,利用数的分解、组合进行加减。数的运算(加、减、乘、除),是儿童学习数的组成之手段,是提高儿童数概念水平的重要方面。  相似文献   
853.
包淳洋  徐秉? 《心理学报》1981,14(3):94-99
一、在猕猴上建立了纯音比配延缓防御反应。一般能完成9秒的延缓反应。最长可延缓15秒。 二、静脉注射LSD对于纯音比配延缓反应有明显的影响,药物注射后延缓反应正确百分率由原来80%以上的水平降到10%—70%。一般24小时后即恢复正常。 三、本文对LSD引起的猕猴纯音延缓反应障碍与LSD引起的临床症状作了比较讨论。  相似文献   
854.
50—90岁成人的短时记忆研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
本工作研究了102例50—90岁成人的短时记忆,其中85例进行了较详细的体格检查。以22例20—25岁青年的记忆检查作为对照,结果看到: 1.在图片自由回忆、逻辑故事识记、数字广度识记三个项目上,记忆都有随年龄增长而逐步减退的明显趋势; 2.识记逻辑故事时,对中心意义的记忆减退较分节记忆减退缓慢,顺背数字的记忆广度随年龄增长而减小较其他项目都要缓慢,表明老年记忆减退有其特点; 3.神经系统和心血管系统的病理性或老年性变化与记忆减退有关。  相似文献   
855.
问 题 人眼对光谱各波长的灵敏度,是产生一种阈限反应或主观相等亮度所需要的辐射能量的倒数。它们原称光谱视亮度,现在我们称之为光谱相对视亮度。即使是等能光谱,各波长引起的相对视亮度也很不相同。具体测量时除绝对阈限法外,一般常用匹配法:使人眼将不同波长的光谱色光分别和一个固定强度的白色标准光作亮度匹配,达  相似文献   
856.
E-learning systems are capable of providing more adaptive and efficient learning experiences for learners than traditional classroom settings. A key component of such systems is the learning policy. The learning policy is an algorithm that designs the learning paths or rather it selects learning materials for learners based on information such as the learners’ current progresses and skills, learning material contents. In this article, the authors address the problem of finding the optimal learning policy. To this end, a model for learners’ hierarchical skills in the E-learning system is first developed. Based on the hierarchical skill model and the classical cognitive diagnosis model, a framework to model various mastery levels related to hierarchical skills is further developed. The optimal learning path in consideration of the hierarchical structure of skills is found by applying a model-free reinforcement learning method, which does not require any assumption about learners’ learning transition processes. The effectiveness of the proposed framework is demonstrated via simulation studies.  相似文献   
857.
Despite numerous studies demonstrating that authoritarian leadership and benevolent leadership exert incompatible influence on an individual’s creativity, the combined effects of authoritarian leadership and benevolent leadership on an individual’s creativity and the related mechanisms have yet to be explained. This study tests a model that considers the combined effects of authoritarian and benevolent leadership on graduate student creativity in the universities in China, while also examining the mediating role of the intrinsic motivation of graduate students. Multisource data were collected from 297 graduate students in 60 university scientific research teams in China. The results show that when authoritarian leadership and benevolent leadership are in congruence, the intrinsic motivation of graduate students and their creativity increase as supervisor authoritarianism and benevolence increases. When authoritarian leadership and benevolent leadership are in discrepancy, the intrinsic motivation of graduate students is higher when low supervisor authoritarianism is combined with high benevolence; however, the findings also show that low authoritarian leadership combined with high benevolent leadership would not increase graduate student creativity. The relationship between authoritarian–benevolent leadership and graduate student creativity is partially mediated by intrinsic motivation. The theoretical and practical implications of the results are discussed.  相似文献   
858.
Prior work suggests that follower and leader risk orientation is positively associated with follower creativity. We suggest that this view is oversimplified and propose that follower creativity can be stimulated when leader and follower have diverging risk orientations. We, therefore, apply a configurational approach to creativity, evaluating varying combinations of leader and follower risk orientation on follower creativity. Across two field studies, we demonstrate that (a) follower creativity increases as leaders’ and followers’ risk orientations become more discrepant (i.e., incongruent); (b) follower creativity is higher when leader–follower dyads are congruent at moderate levels of risk orientation compared to congruence at the extremes (i.e., low and high levels); (c) follower‐experienced intellectual stimulation mediates the relationship between leader–follower risk orientation incongruence and congruence and follower creativity; and (d) that leader authority openness moderates the indirect effect of leader–follower risk orientation incongruence on creativity via follower‐experienced intellectual stimulation. Theoretical and practical implications specific to creativity and leader–follower relationships are discussed.  相似文献   
859.
Diagnostic classification models (DCMs) are important statistical tools in cognitive diagnosis. In this paper, we consider the issue of their identifiability. In particular, we focus on one basic and popular model, the DINA model. We propose sufficient and necessary conditions under which the model parameters are identifiable from the data. The consequences, in terms of the consistency of parameter estimates, of fulfilling or failing to fulfill these conditions are illustrated via simulation. The results can be easily extended to the DINO model through the duality of the DINA and DINO models. Moreover, the proposed theoretical framework could be applied to study the identifiability issue of other DCMs.  相似文献   
860.
The authors’ goals were to evaluate the psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Child Social Preference Scale (CSPS; R. J. Coplan, K. Prakash, K. O'Neil, & M. Armer, 2004) and examine the links between both shyness and unsociability and indices of socioemotional functioning in young Chinese children. Participants included of two samples recruited from kindergarten classes in two public schools in Shanghai, China. Both samples included children 3–5 years old (Sample 1: n = 350, Mage = 4.72 years, SD = 0.58 years; Sample 2: n = 129, Mage = 4.40 years, SD = 0.58 years). In both samples, mothers rated children's social withdrawal using the newly created Chinese version of the CSPS, and in Sample 2, teachers also provided ratings of socioemotional functioning. Consistent with previous findings from other cultures, results from factor analyses suggested a 2-factor model for the CSPS (shyness and unsociability) among young children in China. In contrast to findings from North America, child shyness and unsociability were associated with socioemotional difficulties in kindergarten. Some gender differences were also noted. Results are discussed in terms of the assessment and implications of social withdrawal in early childhood in China.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号