全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4132篇 |
免费 | 725篇 |
国内免费 | 226篇 |
专业分类
5083篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 5篇 |
2024年 | 63篇 |
2023年 | 62篇 |
2022年 | 89篇 |
2021年 | 137篇 |
2020年 | 173篇 |
2019年 | 143篇 |
2018年 | 183篇 |
2017年 | 170篇 |
2016年 | 178篇 |
2015年 | 133篇 |
2014年 | 150篇 |
2013年 | 348篇 |
2012年 | 262篇 |
2011年 | 299篇 |
2010年 | 235篇 |
2009年 | 256篇 |
2008年 | 288篇 |
2007年 | 344篇 |
2006年 | 348篇 |
2005年 | 246篇 |
2004年 | 185篇 |
2003年 | 130篇 |
2002年 | 103篇 |
2001年 | 54篇 |
2000年 | 57篇 |
1999年 | 57篇 |
1998年 | 55篇 |
1997年 | 45篇 |
1996年 | 33篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有5083条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
摘要 词汇语音的学习受哪些因素的影响是心理语言学研究中一个重要的问题。语音回路理论认为,语音短时记忆在词汇语音的学习过程中发挥着关键性的作用,语音短时记忆能力越好越有助于词汇的学习;语音敏感性假说认为,语音意识是影响儿童词汇发展及学习的重要因素;另外一些研究者认为,语音知识可能发挥着更为基础和关键的作用,最近的研究则更加强调几种因素如何相互作用影响词汇语音的学习。本文最后提出了该领域需要进一步研究的问题。 相似文献
112.
113.
114.
115.
以345名12~17岁初中阶段流动儿童为被试,采用问卷法考察学校道德氛围感知在流动儿童父母心理控制与道德推脱关系中的中介作用,以及性别的调节作用。结果表明:(1)父母心理控制正向预测流动儿童道德推脱;(2)学校道德氛围感知在父母心理控制和流动儿童道德推脱之间起到部分中介作用。(3)性别在父母心理控制与学校道德氛围感知间起调节作用,性别调节了中介模型的前半路径,具体而言,与男孩相较,女孩的学校道德氛围感知受到父母心理控制的影响作用更加显著。 相似文献
116.
117.
118.
The experiments described in the lead articles replicate findings from previous studies of development of knowledge about balance scales, add several new findings, and raise four key questions: (a) How can rule use best be assessed? (b) How can we reconcile systematic use of rules with variable use of strategies? (c) When do children begin to use rules? and (d) How do children generate new rules? In this Reflection, we summarize current understanding of development of knowledge about balance scales and consider each of the four questions. 相似文献
119.
Unimanual left-right responses to up-down stimuli show a stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) effect for which the preferred mapping varies as a function of response eccentricity. Responses made in the right hemispace and, to a lesser extent, at a midline position, are faster with the up-right/down-left mapping than with the up-left/down-right mapping, but responses made in the left hemispace are faster with the up-left/down-right mapping. Also, for responses at the midline position, the preferred mapping switches when the hand is placed in a supine posture instead of the more usual prone posture. The response eccentricity effect can be explained in terms of correspondence of asymmetrically coded stimulus and response features, but it is not obvious whether the hand posture effect can be explained in a similar manner. The present study tested the implications of a hypothesis that the body of the hand provides a frame of reference with respect to which the response switch is coded as left or right. As was predicted by this hand referent hypothesis, Experiment 1 showed that the influence of hand posture (prone and supine) on orthogonal SRC was additive with that of response location. In Experiment 2, the location of the switch relative to the hand was varied by having subjects use either a normal grip in which the switch was held between the thumb and the index finger or a grip in which it was held between the little and the ring fingers. The magnitudes of the mapping preferences varied as a function of the grip and hand posture in a manner consistent with the hand referent hypothesis. 相似文献
120.
Examination of relationships among trait-like individual differences, state-like individual differences, and learning performance 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Several authors (e.g., J.T. Austin & H.J. Klein, 1996; R. Kanfer, 1990b, 1992) have urged researchers to examine comprehensive models of distal individual differences as predictors of proximal motivational processes and performance. Two field studies in an academic setting tested a model of relationships among trait-like individual differences (cognitive ability, general self-efficacy, and goal orientation), state-like individual differences (state anxiety, task-specific self-efficacy, and goals), and learning performance. Most hypothesized relationships among these constructs received support when tested on 2 samples, when examining different performance episodes, and when using different goal orientation and state-anxiety measures. In general, state-like individual differences were found to mediate the relationships between trait-like individual differences and learning performance. Implications of these results are discussed and suggestions for future research are provided. 相似文献