全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3732篇 |
免费 | 917篇 |
国内免费 | 294篇 |
专业分类
4943篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 78篇 |
2023年 | 66篇 |
2022年 | 78篇 |
2021年 | 133篇 |
2020年 | 182篇 |
2019年 | 119篇 |
2018年 | 144篇 |
2017年 | 146篇 |
2016年 | 119篇 |
2015年 | 118篇 |
2014年 | 131篇 |
2013年 | 299篇 |
2012年 | 242篇 |
2011年 | 290篇 |
2010年 | 260篇 |
2009年 | 254篇 |
2008年 | 268篇 |
2007年 | 396篇 |
2006年 | 362篇 |
2005年 | 310篇 |
2004年 | 195篇 |
2003年 | 146篇 |
2002年 | 96篇 |
2001年 | 72篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 74篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 35篇 |
1996年 | 33篇 |
1995年 | 32篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有4943条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
本文从1904年《东浙杂志》发表的一篇“金华人之拜草木”一文的评论到2005年11月7日《金华晚报》发表的另一篇评论“有病不看医生,去求‘樟树娘娘…,虽相隔一百多年,但之迷信、愚昧,同出一辙。时至今日,愚昧、迷信,不但不减,反而花样翻新。“理疗内裤”、“开光内裤”,更以广告形式大加宣扬。看来“开展中国特色社会主义理论体系宣传普及活动。推动当代马克思主义大众化”是当务之急. 相似文献
112.
本研究采用调查分析法、访谈法,对0-6岁儿童家长与教师的早期教育观念进行了调查.研究发现,这两类群体认为儿童在0-6岁期问最需要关注的是健康的身体素质、诚实守信的道德品质、坚持与专注的学习品质;不同年龄阶段儿童(0-3岁、4-6岁)需要关注的素质与早期教育方式因他们身心发展特点不同而有所不同;(3)青年人与中年人评定的结果基本一致,教师与家长在多个项目的评定上呈现出差异.研究的结果对早期教育的导向有所启示. 相似文献
113.
114.
AbstractA general modeling framework of response accuracy and response times is proposed to track skill acquisition and provide additional diagnostic information on the change of latent speed in a learning environment. This framework consists of two types of models: a dynamic response model that captures the response accuracy and the change of discrete latent attribute profile upon factors such as practice, intervention effects, and other latent and observable covariates, and a dynamic response time model that describes the change of the continuous response latency due to change of latent attribute profile. These two types of models are connected through a parameter, describing the change rate of the latent speed through the learning process, and a covariate defined as a function of the latent attribute profile. A Bayesian estimation procedure is developed to calibrate the model parameters and measure the latent variables. The estimation algorithm is evaluated through several simulation studies under various conditions. The proposed models are applied to a real data set collected through a spatial rotation diagnostic assessment paired with learning tools. 相似文献
115.
Jihong Zhang Jonathan Templin Catherine E. Mintz 《Multivariate behavioral research》2020,55(1):161-162
116.
We conducted two experimental studies with between-subjects and within-subjects designs to investigate the item response process for personality measures administered in high- versus low-stakes situations. Apart from assessing measurement validity of the item response process, we examined predictive validity; that is, whether or not different response models entail differential selection outcomes. We found that ideal point response models fit slightly better than dominance response models across high- versus low-stakes situations in both studies. Additionally, fitting ideal point models to the data led to fewer items displaying differential item functioning compared to fitting dominance models. We also identified several items that functioned as intermediate items in both the faking and honest conditions when ideal point models were fitted, suggesting that ideal point model is “theoretically” more suitable across these contexts for personality inventories. However, the use of different response models (dominance vs. ideal point) did not have any substantial impact on the validity of personality measures in high-stakes situations, or the effectiveness of selection decisions such as mean performance or percent of fakers selected. These findings are significant in that although prior research supports the importance and use of ideal point models for measuring personality, we find that in the case of personality faking, though ideal point models seem to have slightly better measurement validity, the use of dominance models may be adequate with no loss to predictive validity. 相似文献
117.
118.
Studies have shown that performance-dependent monetary rewards facilitate visual perception. However, no study has examined whether such a positive effect is limited to the rewarded task or may be generalized to other tasks. In the current study, two groups of people were asked to perform two visual perception tasks, one being a reward-relevant task and the other being a reward-irrelevant task. For the reward-relevant task, the experimental group received performance-dependent monetary rewards, whereas the control group did not. For the reward-irrelevant task, both groups were not rewarded. The two tasks were randomly intermixed trial by trial (Experiment 1) or presented block by block (Experiment 2) or session by session (Experiments 3a, 3b, and 3c). Results showed that performance-dependent monetary rewards improved participants' performance on the relevant task in all experiments and impaired their performance on the irrelevant task in Experiments 2, 3a, 3b, and 3c. These results suggested that monetary rewards might incur a cost on reward-irrelevant tasks. Finally, the benefit of monetary rewards disappeared when they were no longer provided during the final session. This is the first study that reveals both the bright and dark sides of the performance-dependent monetary rewards in visual perception. 相似文献
119.
Advertisements using assertive language are commonly seen in marketing communications, yet assertive language has often been shown to be ineffective in communication and even decrease compliance. Recent research began to examine factors that influence the effectiveness of assertive advertising messages, but little research has studied the potential moderating role of consumer‐related factors. The current research fills this gap by investigating the moderating role of consumers’ power. Across five studies, the authors find that for high‐power consumers, assertive ads are effective in promoting want products but ineffective in promoting should products. For low‐power consumers, however, assertive ads are effective in promoting should products but ineffective in promoting want products. Moreover, the authors show that this pattern of results is driven (mediated) by consumers’ perception of the fit between the advertising message and the advertised product. Theoretical and practical implications of the findings are discussed. 相似文献
120.
Joseph P. Rennie Mengya Zhang Erin Hawkins Joe Bathelt Duncan E. Astle 《Developmental science》2020,23(4)
We used two simple unsupervised machine learning techniques to identify differential trajectories of change in children who undergo intensive working memory (WM) training. We used self‐organizing maps (SOMs)—a type of simple artificial neural network—to represent multivariate cognitive training data, and then tested whether the way tasks are represented changed as a result of training. The patterns of change we observed in the SOM weight matrices implied that the processes drawn upon to perform WM tasks changed following training. This was then combined with K‐means clustering to identify distinct groups of children who respond to the training in different ways. Firstly, the K‐means clustering was applied to an independent large sample (N = 616, Mage = 9.16 years, range = 5.16–17.91 years) to identify subgroups. We then allocated children who had been through cognitive training (N = 179, Mage = 9.00 years, range = 7.08–11.50 years) to these same four subgroups, both before and after their training. In doing so, we were able to map their improvement trajectories. Scores on a separate measure of fluid intelligence were predictive of a child's improvement trajectory. This paper provides an alternative approach to analysing cognitive training data that go beyond considering changes in individual tasks. This proof‐of‐principle demonstrates a potentially powerful way of distinguishing task‐specific from domain‐general changes following training and of establishing different profiles of response to training. 相似文献