全文获取类型
收费全文 | 500篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
504篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 5篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 51篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 16篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有504条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Doris Aaronson Nancy Markowitz Hollis Shapiro 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1971,9(4):338-344
For a fixed presentation rate, the ratio of speech-to-pause time was varied in three experiments. Ss recalled seven-digit sequences or monitored for item or order information in addition to recall. Removing 33% of the speech and substituting pause time improved recall accuracy and monitoring reaction times. The data suggest that loss of order information in recall may result from cumulative perceptual delays when adequate pause time is unavailable. 相似文献
52.
Helen Brandhorst Krumboltz Johanna Shapiro 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1979,57(8):415-418
After consciousness raising, what? Women who want to alter their socialized responses may learn the necessary skills from counselors. Teaching behavioral self-management principles in the context of women's sex role enculturation is a preventive as well as corrective approach to helping women develop increased personal effectiveness. It is also an important option for those who do not need therapy or who have found other skill training insufficient. 相似文献
53.
R A Schmidt D E Young S Swinnen D C Shapiro 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1989,15(2):352-359
Summary knowledge of results (KR) involves the presentation KR for each of a set of trials (e.g., 10) only after the last trial in the set has been completed. Earlier, Lavery (1962) showed that, relative to providing KR after each trial, a 20-trial summary KR was detrimental to performance in a practice phase with KR present but was beneficial for a no-KR retention test. Using a relatively simple ballistic-timing task, we examined summary lengths of 1 (essentially KR after every trial), 5, 10, and 15 trials, searching for an inverted-U relationship between summary length and retention performance as predicated by a guidance hypothesis for KR. During acquisition when KR was present and being manipulated, all groups showed improvements in performance across practice, while increased summary lengths generally depressed performance. However, in a delayed no-KR retention test, there was an inverse relation between the summary length in acquisition and absolute constant error on the retention test. A guidance hypothesis is favored to explain how, relative to immediate KR, long KR summaries can provide detrimental effects in acquisition while enhancing retention performance. 相似文献
54.
In the present study, we examined letter detection in very frequent function-word sequences. It has been claimed that such sequences are processed in a unitized manner, thus preempting access to their constituent letters. In contrast, we showed that letter detection in the words for and the (1) was no more difficult when the words appeared in adjacent locations in a sentence (familiar) than when they appeared apart (less familiar sequence) and (2) was contingent upon the words' syntactic roles within the phrase. Thus, letter detection in for was easier when the sequence was separated by a clause boundary than when the words were part of the same clause. The advantage derived from clause separation was strongest when a comma divided clauses. These results challenge the unitization account of the "missing-letter" effect in common phrases and support a position where this phenomenon is seen to reflect the extraction of phrase structure during reading. 相似文献
55.
Evaluated the effect of varied physician affect on subject recall, anxiety, and perceptions in a simulated tense and ambiguous medical situation. Forty women at risk for breast cancer viewed videotapes of an oncologist presenting--with either worried or nonworried affect--mammogram results. Although the mammogram results and the oncologist were the same in both presentation, analyses indicated that, compared to the women receiving the results from a nonworried physician, the women receiving the results from a worried physician recalled significantly less information, perceived the clinical situation as significantly more severe, reported significantly higher levels of state anxiety, and had significantly higher pulse rates. These results suggest that physician affect plays a critical role in patient reaction to medical information. Implications for compliance research, patient satisfaction, and physician training are discussed. 相似文献
56.
We present a study wherein a severe Broca's aphasic patient was trained to learn symbols representing both pure transitive and dative predicates--predicates differing in argument structure--in a visually based artificial language (c-ViC). We found a decrease in performance when two symbols, rather than one, were used to depict these "verbs." However, this decrease in performance was more pronounced for symbols representing pure transitive verbs--those that allow only one argument structure--than for symbols representing dative verbs--those that allow two different argument structures. Also, dative "verbs" yielded better performance when they were inserted in more complex, three-argument "sentences" than when they were inserted in two-argument "sentences." The opposite pattern was found for pure transitives. These results are discussed in terms of our claim that argument structure serves as a point of connection between linguistic information and non-linguistic visual information and in terms of the possibility that argument structure entries are shaped by the form in which visual information is parsed. 相似文献
57.
An on-line assessment of causal reasoning during comprehension 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Charles P. Bloom Charles R. Fletcher Paul Van Den Broek Laura Reitz Brian P. Shapiro 《Memory & cognition》1990,18(1):65-71
Fletcher and Bloom (1988) have argued that as readers read narratives, clause by clause, they repeatedly focus their attention on the last preceding clause that contains antecedents but no consequences in the text. This strategy allows them to discover a causal path linking the text's opening to its final outcome while minimizing the number of times long-term memory must be searched for missing antecedents or consequences. In order to test this hypothesis, we examined the reading times of 25 subjects for each clause of eight simple narrative texts. The results show that: (1) causal links between clauses that co-occur in short-term memory (as predicted by the strategy) increase the time required to read the second clause; (2) potential causal links between clauses that never co-occur in short-term memory (again as predicted by the strategy) have no effect on reading time; and (3) reinstatement searches are initiated at the end of sentences that are causally unrelated to the contents of short-term memory or that contain clauses that satisfy goals no longer in short-term memory. These results support the claim that subjects engage in a form of causal reasoning when they read simple narrative texts. 相似文献
58.
Summary Familiarity effects in visual letter processing were examined by means of a mutilation detection task. The uppercase E was used as the non-mutilated letter. One or two horizontal bars of the E were delected, to produce an upright or inverted F or L. These four mutilations of the E were assigned to the same response. In Experiment 1, brief exposure of the stimuli was followed by a backward mask. Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1, except that the display duration of the stimulus was increased and the backward mask was omitted. In Experiment 3, the overall luminance of the stimuli (luminance per point times length of the constituent line segments) was held constant. In Experiment 4, the upright and inverted non-E letter occurred in different blocks to encourage a normal (upright) letter processing strategy in the upright letter blocks. Accuracy (Experiment 1) and mean correct response times (all experiments) were not different for the upright and inverted F or for the upright and inverted L. These findings and converging results from other studies indicate that the higher familiarity of the upright letters did not aid feature extraction. 相似文献
59.
60.
Nigel Mathers Geraldine Shipton David Shapiro 《British Journal of Guidance & Counselling》1993,21(3):310-318
An evaluation was conducted of short-term counselling in a higher education setting. The main objective was to measure client scores on a shortened version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) pre- and post- counselling. Forty-five matched pairs of GHQ-12 scores were compared. There was a highly significant reduction in scores post-counselling. The evidence suggests that the 'great pretender' evaluation can be challenged using standard quantitative measures such as the GHQ-12. It is concluded that counselling in higher education may be addressing long-standing distress in members of the student population. 相似文献