首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2579篇
  免费   115篇
  国内免费   41篇
  2023年   20篇
  2022年   18篇
  2021年   26篇
  2020年   56篇
  2019年   66篇
  2018年   93篇
  2017年   80篇
  2016年   72篇
  2015年   72篇
  2014年   58篇
  2013年   269篇
  2012年   107篇
  2011年   102篇
  2010年   80篇
  2009年   67篇
  2008年   91篇
  2007年   101篇
  2006年   89篇
  2005年   63篇
  2004年   73篇
  2003年   48篇
  2002年   52篇
  2001年   123篇
  2000年   118篇
  1999年   78篇
  1998年   18篇
  1997年   28篇
  1996年   22篇
  1994年   19篇
  1992年   40篇
  1991年   54篇
  1990年   40篇
  1989年   43篇
  1988年   42篇
  1987年   28篇
  1986年   19篇
  1985年   23篇
  1984年   22篇
  1983年   18篇
  1981年   15篇
  1979年   20篇
  1976年   15篇
  1975年   13篇
  1974年   16篇
  1973年   16篇
  1971年   17篇
  1970年   14篇
  1969年   15篇
  1968年   16篇
  1967年   16篇
排序方式: 共有2735条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
991.
As a key part of the brain’s reward system, midbrain dopamine neurons are thought to generate signals that reflect errors in the prediction of reward. However, recent evidence suggests that “upstream” brain areas may make important contributions to the generation of prediction error signals. To address this issue, we recorded neural activity in midbrain reticular formation (MRNm) while rats performed a spatial working memory task. A large proportion of these neurons displayed positive and negative reward prediction error-related activity that was strikingly similar to that observed in dopamine neurons. Therefore, MRNm may be a source of reward prediction error signals.The capacity of an organism to respond appropriately to environmental stimuli depends on the ability to detect changes in the outcome of its behavior. The mesocorticolimbic dopamine system is thought to be central to this function (Wise 2004; Fields et al. 2007). Dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) increase activity relative to the presentation of cues that predict rewards and rewards of greater value than expected, and decrease activity relative to rewards of less value than predicted (Nakahara et al. 2004; Bayer and Glimcher 2005; Pan et al. 2005; Tobler et al. 2005). This activity is thought to be involved in a computation about errors in the prediction of reward (Schultz and Dickinson 2000) that can be used to correct behavior. A central issue relevant to the behavioral and computational interpretation of dopamine signals is whether prediction error signals are generated by dopamine neurons, per se, or by cells in “upstream” brain areas.Recent data suggest that brain areas afferent to dopamine neurons generate, or participate in, reward prediction error computations. Lateral habenula, which has been shown to inhibit the activity of VTA and SNc dopamine neurons (Herkenham and Nauta 1979; Christoph et al. 1986), has recently been identified as a potential source of reward prediction error signals (Matsumoto and Hikosaka 2007). A similar finding has been demonstrated in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPTg), which is an important regulator of dopamine neuron activity (Floresco et al. 2003). PPTg neural responses varied according to whether or not the animal received expected rewards (Kobayashi and Okada 2007).As part of a larger study investigating the role of VTA in context-dependent spatial working memory (C.B. Puryear, M.J. Kim, and S.J.Y. Mizumori, in prep.), we recorded the activity of neurons in the magnocellular region of the midbrain reticular formation (MRNm according to the method of Swanson [2003]) at the level of the diencephalon. The reticular formation is thought to be important for modulating arousal and vigilance levels necessary for attending to and acting upon salient stimuli (Pragay et al. 1978; Mesulam 1981), and it has recently been shown that this portion of the reticular formation provides glutamatergic input to VTA (Geisler et al. 2007). Thus, MRNm is in a prime position to modulate the activity of VTA dopamine neurons when the outcome of behavior does not meet expectations and therefore may be a source of reward prediction error signals. Accordingly, we investigated whether MRNm neurons exhibited reward related activity, and whether this activity was related to the ability to predict acquisition of reward.Four male Long-Evans rats (4–6 mo old from Simonson Laboratory, Gilroy, CA) were housed individually in Plexiglas cages in a temperature- and humidity-controlled environment (12:12 h light:dark). Rats were provided with food and water ad libitum for 5 d prior to being handled daily and reduced to 85% of ad libitum feeding weights. Animal care and use was conducted according to University of Washington’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines.Rats were habituated to the testing environment and trained to perform a differential reward, win-shift spatial working memory task using radial maze procedures reported previously (Pratt and Mizumori 2001; C.B. Puryear, M.J. Kim, and S.J.Y. Mizumori, in prep.). Briefly, prior to the start of each trial, the end of each of the eight maze arms was baited with either a large (five drops) or small (one drop) amount of chocolate milk on alternating arms. Maze arms containing large or small amounts of reward were counterbalanced across rats and held constant throughout training. Trials started with a sample phase by presenting four maze arms (two large and two small reward arms; individually and randomly selected) to the rat. Immediately after presentation of the fourth arm, a test phase began by making all maze arms accessible so the rat could collect the remaining rewards. The trial ended once all arms were visited. The rat was then confined to the center of the maze for a 2-min intertrial interval. Arm re-entries were counted as errors. Once the rat was able to perform at 15 trials in less than 1 h for seven consecutive days, recording electrodes were surgically implanted.Details concerning the construction of recording electrodes and microdrives and surgical procedures can be found in previous works (McNaughton et al. 1983; Puryear et al. 2006; C.B. Puryear, M.J. Kim, and S.J.Y. Mizumori, in prep.). Briefly, rats were chronically implanted with either eight stereotrodes (four/hemisphere) or four tetrodes (two/hemisphere) made from 25-μm lacquer-coated tungsten wire (California Fine Wire), centered on the following coordinates relative to bregma: −5.25 mm posterior, 0.7 mm lateral, 6 mm ventral. One week of free feeding was allowed for rats to recover from surgery before recording experiments began.Recordings were performed as described previously (Puryear et al. 2006; C.B. Puryear, M.J. Kim, and S.J.Y. Mizumori, in prep.). If no clear spontaneous neural activity was encountered, electrodes were lowered in ∼25-μm increments (up to 175 μm/d) until unambiguous, isolatable units were observed. Single units were isolated from multiunit records using standard cluster-cutting software (MClust; A.D. Redish, University of Wisconsin). A template-matching algorithm was also used to facilitate separation of unique spike waveforms. We only included cells that had a high signal-to-noise ratio (>3:1), exhibited stable clusters throughout the recording session, and had clear refractory periods in the interspike interval histograms following cluster cutting.The final position of each stereotrode was marked by passing a 25-μA current through each recording wire for 25 sec while rats were under 5% isofluorane anesthesia. Rats were then given an overdose of sodium pentobarbital and transcardially perfused (0.9% buffered saline, followed by 10% formalin). Electrodes were retracted, and the brain was removed and allowed to sink in 30% sucrose-formalin. Coronal sections (40 μm) were sliced with a cryostat and stained with cresyl violet. Recording locations were verified by comparing depth measurements and reconstructions of the electrode tracks. Only cells determined to be located in MRNm (Swanson 2003) were considered for analysis.Rats were well trained on the spatial working memory task, committing 0.86 ± 0.2 (mean ± SEM) errors per trial during the first five trials (baseline trials). Importantly, rats demonstrated the ability to discriminate large and small reward locations. There was a significant negative correlation between the first four test phase arm choices (i.e., first, second, third, and fourth arm choice) and the probability that the arm chosen contained a large reward (Fig. 1B; Spearman’s ρ = −0.65, P < 0.001), indicating that rats reliably visited large reward arms before small reward arms during the test phase of each trial.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Histology and basic firing properties of MRNm neurons. (A) Distribution of cells localized to MRNm. Each dot may represent the location of more than one neuron. Coronal slices adapted from Swanson (2003) (reprinted with permission from Academic Press ©2003). (B) Rats displayed preference for arms that contained large amounts of reward. Plotted is the average probability of choosing a large reward arm during the first four arm choices of the test phase of each trial. Error bars represent SEM. (C) Distribution of average firing rates and spike duration of MRNm neurons. Most cells fired less than 10 spikes/sec and exhibited waveform durations between 1.5 and 2.0 msec. (D) Examples of two MRNm neurons. Top row shows their average waveform on each wire of the tetrode. Scale bar = 1 msec. Middle and bottom rows depict their interspike interval and autocorrelation histograms, respectively.A total of 18 cells localized to MRNm were recorded while rats performed the task. Of these, one cell was omitted from analysis due to a very low average firing rate (∼0.2 spikes/sec), yielding 17 cells included in the following analyses. Figure 1A depicts the distribution of cells localized to MRNm. These cells exhibited a range of average firing rates, spike durations (defined as the time from the start to the end of the action potential) (Fig. 1C), and firing patterns (for representative interspike interval and autocorrelation histograms, see Fig. 1D).Reward-related neural activity was obtained by placing rewards in small metal cups mounted to the end of each maze arm and connected to the recording equipment (custom designed by Neuralynx, Inc.), which served as “lick detectors.” An event marker was automatically inserted into the data stream when the rat licked the cup, providing an instantaneous measurement of the time the rat first obtained reward.In order to determine whether MRNm neurons exhibited significant reward-related activity, peri-event time histograms (PETHs) were constructed (50 msec bins, ±2.5 sec around each reward event). A cell was considered to have a significant excitatory reward response if it passed the following two criteria: (1) The cell had a peak firing rate within ±150 msec of reward acquisition and (2) the peak rate was >150% of its average firing rate for the block of trials. These criteria were applied to PETHs collapsed across reward amounts and separately for large and small reward events. Overall, 47% (eight of 17) of MRNm neurons were found to exhibit significant excitatory responses upon acquisition of reward (Fig. 2D). Of these cells, most (88%, seven of eight) were found to fire relative to acquisition of only large rewards (e.g., Fig. 2A–C), while the remaining neuron fired relative to acquisition of both reward amounts. No cells were found to fire preferentially to acquisition of only small rewards. Aspects of animals’ movement (e.g., velocity) were not found to be a major contributor to the firing patterns of MRNm neurons during performance of the spatial working memory task (data not shown). Therefore, it appears that MRNm unit activity is predominantly biased to represent higher reward values.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Reward-related activity of MRNm neurons. (A) Peri-event time histograms of one cell that exhibited a short-latency, excitatory response upon acquisition of rewards (t(0), bin width = 50 msec). Left histogram shows only a modest excitatory response when considering all rewards together. However, top and bottom right histograms show that the reward-related firing occurred upon acquisition of large and not small rewards. Gray-shaded areas indicate time periods analyzed for significant increases in firing rate. (B) Population summary of the proportion of MRNm neurons that demonstrated significant reward-related activity.In order to determine whether MRNm reward-related activity was associated with reward prediction, we tested unit responses to unexpected alterations of reward outcome or elimination of visuospatial information important for reward prediction. To do this, we allowed the rat to perform a second block of five trials with either the locations of large and small rewards switched (reward location switch condition), with two rewards (one large and one small, randomly selected) omitted from the study phase of each trial (reward omission condition), or with the maze room lights extinguished (darkness condition). Importantly, each of these manipulations created situations in which reward prediction errors likely occurred. Overall, these three testing conditions created the following situations, respectively: a mismatch between the locations of large and small rewards, a decreased probability of obtaining a reward, and a situation in which rats are not able to discriminate between arms associated with large and small amounts of reward (C.B. Puryear, M.J. Kim, and S.J.Y. Mizumori, in prep.). Therefore, positive prediction errors could occur when the animal received a large amount of reward on an arm previously associated with a small amount, when the rat retrieved rewards after visiting arms in which reward had been omitted, and when the rat obtained a large reward in darkness. Negative reward prediction errors could occur when the animal received a small amount reward on a maze arm previously associated with a large amount, when the rat visited an arm that did not contain a reward, and when the rat obtained a small reward in darkness.Eight cells with significant responses to large rewards were recorded during these tests (two during reward location switch, four during reward omission conditions, and two during darkness conditions). In order to determine whether the reward manipulations affected the reward-related activity of these cells, a reward activity value (RA) was first calculated, which was the average firing rate in the ±150 msec around the time of acquisition of rewards, expressed as a percentage in change relative to the cell’s average firing rate for each block of trials. These values were normalized to the maximum RA value observed, yielding a normalized RA value for the first and second block of trials (RAn1 and RAn2, respectively). These calculations were made for large and small rewards separately, and for non-rewarded arms in the reward omission condition.We then created scatterplots of RAn’s for each block of trials. If reward-related activity was independent of the expectation of the reward received, RAn1 and RAn2 should be similar in each block of trials. As can been seen in Figure 3A, the reward-related activity was consistently higher when rats received more reward than expected in the second block of trials. Conversely, Figure 3B clearly shows that neural activity was consistently suppressed when rats received less reward than expected. These differences in reward-related firing were quantified by calculating the distance of each data point to the diagonal (i.e., the reward activity change index, or RACI): Directionality of the change in reward activity was taken into account in order to discern between increases and decreases in firing rate. A one-sample t-test (α = 0.05) indicated that average RACI values were significantly increased when rats received more reward than expected (t(7) = −2.73, P < 0.03) and were significantly decreased when rats received less reward than expected (t(7) = −4.88, P < 0.001). These results are consistent with positive and negative reward prediction error signals, respectively. An example of an MRNm neuron that exhibited both positive and negative prediction error-related activity in the reward location switch condition is depicted in Figure 3D.Open in a separate windowFigure 3.Reward prediction errors in MRNm neurons. (A) Plotted is each neuron’s normalized large reward activity (RAN, defined in text) for each block of trials. The reward activity during block 2 (y-axis) represents activity at times when more reward than expected was obtained. Note that reward-related activity during these times is consistently more robust than during times in which the rat received the expected reward (block 1), indicating that positive reward prediction errors occurred. (B) Plotted are RAN values for rewarded and devalued arms in block 2 (x- and y-axes, respectively). Devalued arms include arms associated with a large amount of reward but baited with a small amount of reward, arms in which reward was omitted, and arms containing small rewards visited in darkness. Note that reward-related activity on devalued arms is consistently suppressed, indicating that negative reward prediction errors occurred. (Symbols in A,B: ● indicates cell recorded in darkness condition; o, cell recorded in reward omission condition; and x, cell recorded in reward location switch condition.) (C) Average changes in reward activity (RACI, defined in text) for times in which the rat obtained more and less reward than expected. Asterisks indicate significant differences (P < 0.05). Error bars indicate SEM. (D) An example of one neuron that did not respond to acquisition of rewards during the first block of trials. When the locations of large and small rewards were switched, however, the cell developed an excitatory response to acquisition of large rewards on arms previously associated with small amounts of reward (positive reward prediction error). Furthermore, the firing of the cell was inhibited upon acquisition of small rewards on arms previously associated with large amounts of reward (negative reward prediction error).We demonstrate here that a large proportion of MRNm neurons may be involved in computations about reward acquisition. Similar to dopamine neurons (Tobler et al. 2005), the majority of reward-related MRNm neurons preferentially fired relative to acquisition of large amounts of reward. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of discriminative reward responses of reticular formation neurons and highlights a novel role for the reticular formation in reward value representations. Furthermore, these data suggest that MRNm neurons represent similar reward prediction error signals as dopamine neurons (Nakahara et al. 2004; Bayer and Glimcher 2005; Pan et al. 2005; Tobler et al. 2005). It is important to note that in this initial sample, there was remarkable overall consistency and reliability of the positive and negative reward prediction error signals by MRNm neurons. This is similar to the homogeneity of dopamine neuron responses, suggesting that reward prediction may be a major function of the overall population of MRNm reward-related neurons. Nevertheless, further parametric studies are necessary to determine whether MRNm neural activity conforms to the same basic firing profiles that have been well-described for dopamine neurons (i.e., predictive cues and reward probabilities).The reticular formation has traditionally been thought to be important for initiating general arousal states. This is in part due to initial reports of changes in unit activity during transitions from sleep to wakefulness (Huttenlocher 1961; Kasamatsu 1970; Manohar et al. 1972). In addition, a more specific role for the reticular formation in attention has been described in primates performing visual discrimination tasks (Pragay et al. 1978; Fabre et al. 1983). This is consistent with reports of sensory neglect following reticular formation lesions (Watson et al. 1974). Together, these foundational data suggest that reticular formation may function to enhance the overall level of arousal and vigilance necessary for attending to and acting upon salient stimuli (Mesulam 1981). Accordingly, changes in reward-related MRNm neuronal activity could provide an important signal indicating that the contingencies of recently executed behaviors have changed.The striking similarity of the reward prediction error signals of MRNm neurons reported here suggests that MRNm, along with brain regions such as lateral habenula (Matsumoto and Hikosaka 2007), pedunculopontine nucleus (PPTg) (Kobayashi and Okada 2007), and dorsal raphé nucleus (Nakamura et al. 2008) may contribute to the generation of reward prediction error signals observed in dopamine neurons. Furthermore, these data suggest the possibility that such signals are a general property of a large network of midbrain structures. Given that the projections from the reticular formation to VTA are glutamatergic (Geisler et al. 2007), it is possible that the changes in reward-related activity of MRNm neurons, in concert with PPTg, could provide an excitatory component of the reward prediction error signal. In combination with inhibitory inputs from lateral habenula, this may then selectively activate dopamine neurons to initiate the coordinated selection of appropriate behaviors in response to changes in reward outcome (Humphries et al. 2007).  相似文献   
992.
Reactivation of stabilized memories returns them to a labile state and causes them to undergo extinction or reconsolidation processes. Although it is well established that administration of glucocorticoids after training enhance consolidation of contextual fear memories, but their effects on post-retrieval processes are not known. In this study, we first asked whether administration of corticosterone after memory reactivation would modulate subsequent expression of memory in rats. Additionally, we examined whether this modulatory action would depend upon the strength of the memory. We also tested the effect of propranolol after memory reactivation. Adult male Wistar rats were trained in a fear conditioning system using moderate (0.4 mA) or high shock (1.5 mA) intensities. For reactivation, rats were returned to the chamber for 90 s 24h later. Immediately after reactivation, rats were injected with corticosterone (1, 3 or 10mg/kg) or vehicle. One, 7 and 14 days after memory reactivation, rats were returned to the context for 5 min, and freezing behavior was scored. The findings indicated that corticosterone when injected after memory reactivation had no significant effect on recall of a moderate memory, but it impaired recall of a strong memory at a dose of 3mg/kg. Propranolol (5mg/kg) given after the reactivation treatment produced a modest impairment that persisted over three test sessions. Further, the results showed that corticosterone, but not propranolol deficit was reversed by a reminder shock. These findings provide evidence that administration of glucocorticoids following memory reactivation reduces subsequent retrieval of strong, but not moderate, contextual conditioned fear memory likely via acceleration of memory extinction. On the other hand, propranolol-induced amnesia may result from blockade of reconsolidation process. Further studies are needed to determine the underlying mechanisms.  相似文献   
993.
Introduction: Evidence from phenomenological, family, genetic, and treatment studies from Western centers have suggested that tic-related obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) could be different from non-tic-related OCD. This study from India investigated the differences in OCD with and without tics, with respect to sociodemographics, symptom profile, and comorbidity, including obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders, to examine whether the clinical profile of tic-related OCD is similar to that reported previously. Methods: Fifty subjects with OCD and tics (chronic motor tics and Tourette syndrome) were compared with 141 OCD subjects without tics. Results: Subjects having OCD with tics tended to be males, and had an earlier onset of illness. They had more of symmetry/aggressive and religious obsessions, and cleaning, ordering/arranging, hoarding, and repeating compulsions and were associated with trichotillomania and hypochondraisis. Stepwise backward (Wald) regression analysis showed that an early age of onset, male gender, aggressive obsessions, cleaning compulsions, and trichotillomania were significantly associated with tic-related OCD. Conclusion: The findings of this study from India are broadly similar to those reported previously from the West indicating the universality of differences in tic- and non-tic-related OCD. Our findings also support the existing evidence that tics contribute to the heterogeneity of OCD.  相似文献   
994.
Research on cognitive models of depression has identified negative cognitive styles and rumination as risk factors for depression. The present study examined the hypothesis that rumination mediates the effect of negative cognitive styles on depression. Specifically, we evaluated the differential effects of two aspects of rumination, characterized by brooding and reflection, on the relationship between negative cognitive styles and level of depressive symptoms. A total of 115 college students and 38 patients suffering from depressive disorders completed a battery of questionnaires measuring levels of depressive symptoms, brooding, reflection, and negative cognitive styles. The results support the notion that there exist two distinct dimensions of rumination and that, of the two, it is brooding and not reflection that mediates the relationship between negative cognitive styles and depression and contributes to its negative outcomes.  相似文献   
995.
BACKGROUND: The effectiveness of a combined methylphenidate/behavioural treatment (BT) versus methylphenidate-only for Chinese children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) was tested in routine clinical practice in Hong Kong. METHODS: A randomized group comparison design was adopted with two treatment conditions (methylphenidate-only; methylphenidate/BT), which lasted for 6 months. There were four assessment time points (pre-treatment, post-treatment, and 6-month and 12-month follow-ups), using the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviours (SWAN) Rating Scale. Parental attitude toward different treatment options of ADHD was also assessed at pre-treatment and post-treatment. Participants included 90 Chinese ADHD children (mean age=8 years, SD=.95). RESULTS: The combination of BT and a low-dose methylphenidate was significantly more effective than methylphenidate-only in reducing ADHD and ODD symptoms at post-treatment. At follow-ups, the benefits of the combined treatment were maintained, while the methylphenidate-only group caught up in improvement in ADHD symptoms. Parents in both treatment conditions showed improved attitude toward medication after the 6-month treatment phase, while their attitude toward BT was positive all along. CONCLUSIONS: This study supported the added benefits of BT, on top of medication, for Chinese ADHD children in routine practice with treatments conducted by regular medical and paramedical staffs.  相似文献   
996.
Previous studies of tactile acuity on the fingertip, using passive touch, have demonstrated an age-related decline in spatial resolution for both sighted and blind subjects. We have reexamined this age dependence with two newly designed tactile-acuity charts that require active exploration of the test symbols. One chart used dot patterns similar to braille, and the other used embossed Landolt rings. Groups of blind braille readers and sighted subjects ranging from 12 to 85 years old were tested in two experiments. We replicated previous findings for sighted subjects by showing an age-related decrease in tactile acuity by nearly 1% per year. Surprisingly, the blind subjects retained high acuity into old age, showing no age-related decline. For the blind subjects, tactile acuity did not correlate with braille reading speed, the amount of daily reading, or the age at which braille was learned. We conclude that when measured with active touch, blind subjects retain high tactile acuity into old age, unlike their aging sighted peers. We propose that blind people's use of active touch in daily activities, not specifically braille reading, results in preservation of tactile acuity across the life span.  相似文献   
997.
Latina breast cancer survivors are not benefiting from advances in psychosocial interventions. Despite their greater breast cancer burden, there is a dearth of information about this population. This qualitative study examined the experiences of 18 immigrant Latina breast cancer survivors along the survivorship continuum, from diagnosis to long-term survivorship. The authors conducted separate focus groups with women in the acute, reentry, and long-term survivorship stages. Through grounded theory analysis, the authors uncovered 5 themes of experience: perceptions of psychological well-being, impact of diagnosis, impact of treatment, need for social support, and development of new attitudes. Some themes were applicable across all survivorship stages, whereas others were more salient at a particular stage. Within themes, the authors identified experiences reported in the general literature (i.e., the debilitating effect of chemotherapy) and distinct cultural experiences (i.e., fear that a cancer diagnosis leads to certain death). Results suggest that treatments tailored for each survivorship stage are more likely to be effective than generic interventions. These findings have the potential to lessen health disparities in utilization of psychosocial treatments by Latina breast cancer survivors.  相似文献   
998.
This study examined the emotional security of kindergarten children in dyadic task‐related interactions with their teachers. In particular, it examined the interrelations between security, task behaviours (persistence and independence), social inhibition, and teachers' support. Participants were 79 kindergartners (mean age=69.7 months) and their 40 regular teachers. Children were selected to approach a normal distribution of social inhibition. Children and teachers were filmed during a dyadic interaction task outside the classroom. Three groups of independent observers rated children's emotional security and their task behaviours, as well as teachers' supportive behaviours. Multilevel modelling revealed a positive link between teachers' support and emotional security. This link suppressed a negative relation between social inhibition and emotional security. In addition, emotional security was positively associated with children's task behaviours and mediated part of the positive link between these behaviours and teachers' support. Finally, security moderated the relation between support and persistence, such that the effect of teachers' support on persistent behaviours was amplified for relatively insecure children. These results highlight the importance of considering emotional security when examining the interactions between kindergarten children and their teachers. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
999.
1000.
Four studies implemented a componential approach to assessing self-enhancement and contrasted this approach with 2 earlier ones: social comparison (comparing self-ratings with ratings of others) and self-insight (comparing self-ratings with ratings by others). In Study 1, the authors varied the traits being rated to identify conditions that lead to more or less similarity between approaches. In Study 2, the authors examined the effects of acquaintance on the conditions identified in Study 1. In Study 3, the authors showed that using rankings renders the self-insight approach equivalent to the component-based approach but also has limitations in assessing self-enhancement. In Study 4, the authors compared the social-comparison and the component-based approaches in terms of their psychological implications; the relation between self-enhancement and adjustment depended on the self-enhancement approach used, and the positive-adjustment correlates of the social-comparison approach disappeared when the confounding influence of the target effect was controlled.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号