首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1204篇
  免费   140篇
  国内免费   252篇
  1596篇
  2024年   7篇
  2023年   26篇
  2022年   51篇
  2021年   43篇
  2020年   56篇
  2019年   53篇
  2018年   61篇
  2017年   56篇
  2016年   55篇
  2015年   39篇
  2014年   64篇
  2013年   96篇
  2012年   74篇
  2011年   103篇
  2010年   69篇
  2009年   71篇
  2008年   93篇
  2007年   115篇
  2006年   89篇
  2005年   91篇
  2004年   58篇
  2003年   40篇
  2002年   28篇
  2001年   25篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   22篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   2篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   3篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   3篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1931年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1596条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
为探讨家庭功能、自我控制与青少年心理复原力的关系及其作用机制,采用家庭功能评定量表、自我控制量表、心理复原力量表对浙江省、山东省、河北省957名中学生进行调查。结果发现:(1)家庭功能对青少年心理复原力具有显著正向预测作用。(2)自我控制在家庭功能和青少年心理复原力之间起到部分中介作用。(3)家庭功能对青少年心理复原力中介过程的前半段路径受到性别调节,相比于女生,男生群体的家庭功能对自我控制的影响更为显著。  相似文献   
82.
In the family‐based prevention science literature, family functioning, defined as positive parenting, parental involvement, family cohesion, family communication, parental monitoring of peers, and parent–adolescent communication, has been shown to ameliorate HIV risk behaviors in Hispanic youth. However, the majority of studies have relied solely on parent or adolescent reports and we know very little about parent–adolescent family functioning discrepancies. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine whether and to what extent parent–adolescent discrepancies in family functioning increased the risk of HIV risk behaviors, including substance use and sexual risk behaviors, and whether these associations vary as a function of acculturation and youth gender. A total of 746 Hispanic 8th grade youth and their primary caregivers were included in the study. Structural equation modeling findings indicate that parent–adolescent family functioning discrepancies are associated with an increased risk of Hispanic adolescent HIV risk behaviors, including lifetime and past 90‐day alcohol and illicit drug use, and early sex initiation. In addition, study findings indicate that results vary by acculturation and youth gender. Findings are discussed in the context of existing family‐based research and practice in preventing and reducing HIV risk behaviors among Hispanic youth and their families.  相似文献   
83.
We examined the impact that measurement artifacts (antithetical items that overlap counterproductive work behavior (CWB), agreement response, halo in supervisor ratings) in organizational citizenship behavior (OCB) measures has on observed relationships between OCB and 15 predictors. Respondents were 146 employees and their supervisors who completed surveys that included OCB measures with and without artifacts. Predictors (conscientiousness, emotional stability, empathy, distributive justice, procedural justice, role ambiguity, role conflict, leader–member exchange, affective organizational commitment, job satisfaction, negative and positive emotion, turnover intention, CWB, and task performance) were chosen that related to OCB in prior meta-analyses. Results with the OCB scale with artifacts were consistent with meta-analyses, whereas results with the artifact-controlled OCB scale were not, suggesting that accepted conclusions about OCB’s relationships with many other variables should be reexamined.  相似文献   
84.
比较雷替曲塞与5-氟尿嘧啶联合奥沙利铂治疗老年晚期结直肠癌的临床疗效及生存分析。经病理明确诊断的老年晚期结直肠癌患者42例分为A、B两组,分别采用雷替曲塞和5-氟尿嘧啶/亚叶酸钙联合奥沙利铂方案化疗,观察两组疗效、不良反应及生存分析。结果A、B两组患者有效率接近,无统计学差异。A、B组疾病进展时间分别是7.5个月和5....  相似文献   
85.
The effects of self–other decision-making on intertemporal choice have been revealed in many studies using a monetary outcome. However, the outcome of intertemporal choice is not restricted to money; time is also a scarce and nonrenewable resource outcome. Thus, we conducted a series of experiments to address the effects of self–other decision-making on time-based intertemporal choice, a type of intertemporal choice that uses time as an outcome. Over the course of three experiments, differences in self–other decision-making were evidenced. Participants who made decisions for others were more likely to prefer the smaller but sooner (SS) option over the larger but later (LL) option and considered the gain of the SS option to be significantly greater than that of the LL option. Participants who made decisions for themselves were likely to prefer the LL option over the SS option. However, they considered the gains of the LL and the SS option to be indifferent. Changing the role of decision-making could affect the ability of individuals to consider the future consequences of their decisions. The effects of self–other decision-making on time-based intertemporal choice could be explained by the accounts of economic reasoning and construal level theory. The findings indicated that the effects of self–other decision-making on time-based intertemporal choice, which could be generated simply by rewording questions, can help individuals make optimal long-term choices without the need for increased control.  相似文献   
86.
The purpose of the present study was to investigate the relationship between brand names and consumers' perceived risk. Hypotheses dealt with whether the presence of a product's brand name affects consumers' perceived risk towards shopping online; whether the familiarity with a brand name influences consumers' perceived risk; and whether online shoppers and non‐shoppers perceive risk towards shopping online differently. Results indicate that the presence or absence of a product's brand name affects online shoppers' perceived risk, but in the opposite direction to that expected. There was no significant difference between online shoppers' perceived risk vis‐à‐vis brand familiarity; however, online shoppers possessed lower perceived risk than non‐shoppers. Implications and limitations are discussed and recommendations for future research are provided. Copyright © 2004 Henry Stewart Publications Ltd.  相似文献   
87.
伪科学(Pseudoscience)一词出现于1844年。我们今天称之为伪科学的许多东西在历史上与科学曾有着千丝万缕的联系,如占星术之于天文学,炼金术之于化学。但是历史上的占星术和炼金术都不是伪科学,只有在科学值得冒充之后,它们有意识地冒充科学才可算是伪科学。面对社会上的日渐泛滥的伪科学,我们不得不提高警惕,利用科学的方法揭露伪科学的真面目。我们迫在眉睫的任务是提高全民族的科学文化素质,提倡科学精神,拒绝伪科学。  相似文献   
88.
在古希腊理性精神与基督教信仰文化交汇的大背景下,奥古斯丁扬弃柏拉图和亚里士多德等思想家的理性自由观,提出了以基督教信仰为基础的信仰自由观。这种自由观展示了信仰、自由意志和理性三者之间富有张力的依存关系,实现了西方自由观的转向,对后来的西方哲学与文化发生了深远影响。这种自由观在德国古典哲学尤其是黑格尔的哲学中得到复兴,但由于从根本上并没有克服自由与决定论之间的困境,现实的人的自由、理性与信仰在黑格尔哲学中都被消解了。即便如此,在当代一些重要的思想家那里,奥古斯丁确立的思维范式依然具有实质性影响。  相似文献   
89.
90.
The main goals of the present study were to investigate the effects of outcome valence on attentional bias toward feedback and examine the internal mechanism of self-defense. We systematically manipulated the outcome valence by providing a bogus score in a rational thinking task and recorded the time positive feedback and negative feedback was viewed in experiment 1. We added the intervention of self-affirmation to examine the self-defense mechanism in experiment 2. The results suggest that (1) in good outcome situations, the participants viewed negative feedback longer than positive feedback. There was a tendency to slightly reduce the attention given to negative feedback in bad outcome situations. (2) Self-affirming participants in bad outcome situations increased their viewing time of negative feedback, which supported the activation of defensiveness.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号