首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   671篇
  免费   123篇
  国内免费   312篇
  2023年   17篇
  2022年   32篇
  2021年   29篇
  2020年   31篇
  2019年   40篇
  2018年   25篇
  2017年   36篇
  2016年   27篇
  2015年   42篇
  2014年   33篇
  2013年   57篇
  2012年   58篇
  2011年   66篇
  2010年   53篇
  2009年   56篇
  2008年   64篇
  2007年   57篇
  2006年   89篇
  2005年   73篇
  2004年   38篇
  2003年   32篇
  2002年   24篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   5篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   1篇
  1970年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1106条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
我国中学生的一般能力因素结构探讨   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
方俐洛  韩聪  凌文辁 《心理学报》1998,31(3):306-312
该研究在对一般能力倾向测验的因素分析基础上,提出了三因素能力结构模型,揭示了中国中学生有一般能力包括。数理能力,知觉能力,心理运动能力三个因素,共8种性向。  相似文献   
92.
初中一,二年级学生的亲子冲突   总被引:25,自引:1,他引:24  
方晓义  董奇 《心理科学》1998,21(2):122-125
本研究选取北京市两所普通中学192名初一和初二学生为被试,通过学生对8个方面亲子冲突的评价,探讨初中生亲子冲突的特点。研究发现,(1)初中生早期的亲子冲突处于较低水平;(2)与母亲的冲突多于与父亲的冲突,女生尤其如此;(3)多数亲子冲突随年级而增加;(4)在多数冲突上,男生与父母的冲突多于女生;(5)亲子冲突最多的三个方面为日常生活安排、学业和家务,而冲突最少的为隐私。  相似文献   
93.
94.
In this study, a priming Stroop paradigm was used to determine whether stereotype activation is unintentional. Priming conditions (priming/no‐priming) and the relationship between priming and target (consistent/inconsistent/no‐relation) were the independent variables; accuracy, reaction time and N400 amplitude were used as dependent variables. The reaction time revealed that stereotype activation is, to some extent, unintentional. Furthermore, the event‐related potenial (ERP) results showed that N400 amplitude was larger for inconsistent conditions than for consistent conditions. This result supported the notion that stereotype activation is an unintentional and automatic process.  相似文献   
95.
96.
Rural to urban migration has become a prominent phenomenon in China. In linking migration to children’s school trajectories, this mixed-methods study explored a range of cultural and contextual factors that contribute to Chinese children’s school wellbeing in the migratory process. The key variables included acculturative attitudes, economic stress, and relationships with family, teachers and peers. The study drew upon survey data from a sample of 301 Chinese migrant students (4th–9th grade) together with in-depth interviews involving ten selected students focusing on how migrant children’s school happiness and productivity are influenced by acculturation, economic hardship, and social relationships. The quantitative analysis indicated the critical role of integrative states in promoting school satisfaction and achievement. Migrant children with better social relations scored higher levels of hope, which in turn contributed to better school outcomes. Contrary to previous findings, economic stress did not hinder school outcomes for these Chinese migrant children, but appeared to act as a positive motivational factor for pursing academic success. The qualitative analysis echoed the quantitative findings, and provided further explanations for the complexity and particularity of these phenomena.  相似文献   
97.
This paper tested the impact of culture on the basic structure of affect. It examined positive affect and negative affect at three levels of activation in the U.S. and China. It used a well-suited tool, the bifactor model, to separate the common variance shared by all types of affect from the variance unique to each. The findings indicate that the structure of affect is different cross-culturally. In the U.S., the most fundamental dimension is defined by moderate activation of positive affect and negative affect, which is the largely bipolar dimension of pleasure-displeasure, whereas in China, it is defined by moderate activation of positive affect only, which is pleasure. In both cultures, negative affect across levels of activation forms another important dimension. Beyond these basic dimensions, secondary dimensions are also identified in both cultures: high activation positive affect, low activation positive affect, and low activation negative affect. They form relatively weak unique factors, independent of the two basic dimensions, suggesting that they are largely mixtures of the basic dimensions.  相似文献   
98.
Although several studies have compared the representation of fractions and decimals, no study has investigated whether fractions and decimals, as two types of rational numbers, share a common representation of magnitude. The current study aimed to answer the question of whether fractions and decimals share a common representation of magnitude and whether the answer is influenced by task paradigms. We included two different number pairs, which were presented sequentially: fraction–decimal mixed pairs and decimal–fraction mixed pairs in all four experiments. Results showed that when the mixed pairs were very close numerically with the distance 0.1 or 0.3, there was a significant distance effect in the comparison task but not in the matching task. However, when the mixed pairs were further apart numerically with the distance 0.3 or 1.3, the distance effect appeared in the matching task regardless of the specific stimuli. We conclude that magnitudes of fractions and decimals can be represented in a common manner, but how they are represented is dependent on the given task. Fractions and decimals could be translated into a common representation of magnitude in the numerical comparison task. In the numerical matching task, fractions and decimals also shared a common representation. However, both of them were represented coarsely, leading to a weak distance effect. Specifically, fractions and decimals produced a significant distance effect only when the numerical distance was larger.  相似文献   
99.
Pigeons were trained to peck a certain number of times on a key that displayed one of several possible numerical symbols. The particular symbol displayed indicated the number of times that the key had to be pecked. The pigeons signalled the completion of the requirement by operating a separate key. They received a food reward for correct response sequences and time-out penalties for incorrect response sequences. In the first experiment nine pigeons learned to allocate 1, 2, 3 or 4 pecks to the corresponding numerosity symbols s 1, s 2, s 3 and s 4 with levels of accuracy well above chance. The second experiment explored the maximum set of numerosities that the pigeons were capable of handling concurrently. Six of the pigeons coped with an s 1s 5 task and four pigeons even managed an s 1s 6 task with performances that were significantly above chance. Analysis of response times suggested that the pigeons were mainly relying on a number-based rather than on a time-based strategy. Received: 11 October 1999 / Accepted after revision: 27 January 2000  相似文献   
100.
幼儿朴素理论发展的研究现状   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
朴素物理学、朴素生物学和朴素心理学是幼儿朴素理论发展的三个核心领域,本体集合、因果原则集合和内部一致的知识体系则是朴素理论的三个重要成分。该文以幼儿对三个核心领域的因果认知发展为中心,以朴素理论三个重要组成成分为线索,对近期幼儿朴素理论发展的研究成果进行了简要的介绍和讨论,并提出进一步研究所需要解决的问题。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号