首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   619篇
  免费   44篇
  2022年   4篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   28篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   59篇
  2012年   25篇
  2011年   45篇
  2010年   16篇
  2009年   18篇
  2008年   27篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   21篇
  2005年   17篇
  2004年   12篇
  2003年   25篇
  2002年   18篇
  2001年   22篇
  2000年   17篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   8篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   7篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   8篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   5篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   5篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   4篇
  1969年   5篇
  1968年   5篇
  1952年   3篇
排序方式: 共有663条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
411.
412.
Predicting how and when consumers will switch from a current familiar brand to a new option is a matter of concern for every level of new product introduction—from brand extensions to “really new” discontinuous innovations. In this article, we build on the innovativeness literature by investigating the degree to which 2 consumer characteristics, the need for cognition (Ncog) and the need for change (Nchange), help explain individuals’ propensity to choose new innovations versus status quo options. We demonstrate that by separating Ncog and Nchange and cross‐classifying individuals based on these attributes, 4 unique patterns of change behavior emerge. A large‐scale choice study was conducted by surveying metropolitan residents about changes in telecommunication services (local, long distance, and cellular). We use a latent class model to uncover the segmentation structure in the choice data, using the constructs as concommitant variables in the segment classification portion of the econometric model. The results show that the predicted theoretical structure explains observed data and can be used to significantly increase the predictive power of models of change behavior.  相似文献   
413.
This paper reports the results of a study of brand selection and loyalty within the 18–24 age group. The study explores brand loyalty behaviour across different product categories, and investigates the dimensions that drive loyalty behaviour within this age group. First, the construct of brand loyalty is defined, followed by an overview of key research in the area. Finally, the study itself is detailed. The study concludes that there is a significant difference in the degree of brand loyalty exhibited by the 18–24‐year‐old respondents across product categories. The dimensions of brand selection also vary by product type. Brand heritage in terms of parental influence was evident in coffee and toothpaste purchase, with brand as a reflection of self‐image being something that is important to clothing brands. Value and variety are important attributes of cereal brand selection. Copyright © 2004 Henry Stewart Publications Ltd.  相似文献   
414.
415.
Recognising a facial expression is more difficult when the expresser's body conveys incongruent affect. Existing research has documented such interference for universally recognisable bodily expressions. However, it remains unknown whether learned, conventional gestures can interfere with facial expression processing. Study 1 participants (N?=?62) viewed videos of people simultaneously producing facial expressions and hand gestures and reported the valence of either the face or hand. Responses were slower and less accurate when the face-hand pairing was incongruent compared to congruent. We hypothesised that hand gestures might exert an even stronger influence on facial expression processing when other routes to understanding the meaning of a facial expression, such as with sensorimotor simulation, are disrupted. Participants in Study 2 (N?=?127) completed the same task, but the facial mobility of some participants was restricted, which disrupted face processing in prior work. The hand-face congruency effect from Study 1 was replicated. The facial mobility manipulation affected males only, and it did not moderate the congruency effect. The present work suggests the affective meaning of conventional gestures is processed automatically and can interfere with face perception, but does not suggest that perceivers rely more on gestures when sensorimotor face processing is disrupted.  相似文献   
416.
Buckingham  Gavin  Parr  Johnny  Wood  Greg  Day  Sarah  Chadwell  Alix  Head  John  Galpin  Adam  Kenney  Laurence  Kyberd  Peter  Gowen  Emma  Poliakoff  Ellen 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2019,26(4):1295-1302
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review - The “uncanny phenomenon” describes the feeling of unease associated with seeing an image that is close to appearing human. Prosthetic hands in...  相似文献   
417.
418.
New Frontiers for Self-Regulation Research in IO Psychology   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Vancouver et Day (2005) font une analyse critique de l’état d’avancement des recherches sur l’autorégulation en psychologie industrielle/organisationnelle (IO). Ils soulèvent des problèmes de validité interne et de construction pour certains concepts clés, tels que le nivcau du but, l’efficacité personnelle, le feedback et la divergence; ils mentionnent aussi qu’il est difficile de préciser quelles sont les facteurs à l’origine des résultats obtenus par les interventions relevant des principes de l’autorégulation. Leurs suggestions concernant les élaborations théoriques, la mesure et la structuration des études sont pertinentes et, si on les met en ?uvre, elles permettront à la psychologie IO d’échapper aux problèmes qu’est susceptible de soulever l’élaboration, sur des bases insuffisantes, d’un corpus de connaissances appliquées. A travers leur approche critique, Vancouver et Day font nécessairement un choix dans la littérature qu’ils analysent et ils ne proposent pas de schéma d’ensemble des recherches sur l’autorégulation en psychologie IO. Vancouver and Day (2005 ) present a critical analysis of the current state of self‐regulation research within IO psychology. Their analysis identifies problems of construct and internal validity for certain key constructs, including goal level, self‐efficacy and feedback discrepancy, and difficulties in detecting the sources of effects in studies of interventions based on self‐regulatory principles. Their suggestions regarding theory development, measurement, and study design are timely and, if implemented, will help IO psychology avoid the potential problems of building a body of applied knowledge based on weak foundations. In their critical approach, Vancouver and Day are necessarily selective in the literature they discuss and they do not provide a comprehensive framework of self‐regulation research in IO psychology.  相似文献   
419.
Different models of lying on personality scales make discrepant predictions on the association between faking and item response time. The current research investigated response time restriction as a method for reducing the influence of faking on personality scale validity. In 3 assessment simulations involving 540 university undergraduates responding to 2 common, psychometrically strong personality inventories, no evidence emerged to indicate that limiting respondents' answering time can attenuate the effects of faking on validity. Results were interpreted as failing to support a simple model of personality test item response dissimulation that predicts that lying takes time. Findings were consistent with models implying that lying involves primitive cognitive processing or that lying may be associated with complex processing that includes both primitive responding and cognitive overrides.  相似文献   
420.
A recent commentary by Meyer (2000) in the Journal of Personality Assessment alleged that Rorschach critic Wood and his colleagues had intentionally published information that they knew to be in error. To substantiate this contention, Meyer's commentary published information that was part of the peer review process at another journal. In this rejoinder, we present factual information that shows we have consistently acted in good faith. This rejoinder suggests that the scientific debate regarding the Comprehensive System for the Rorschach is unlikely to be advanced by speculating about the intentions of Rorschach critics, or by publishing information from the peer review process that is usually kept confidential.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号