全文获取类型
收费全文 | 70125篇 |
免费 | 1212篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
71342篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 875篇 |
2019年 | 1049篇 |
2018年 | 1441篇 |
2017年 | 1439篇 |
2016年 | 1554篇 |
2015年 | 1109篇 |
2014年 | 1364篇 |
2013年 | 6209篇 |
2012年 | 2444篇 |
2011年 | 2485篇 |
2010年 | 1533篇 |
2009年 | 1554篇 |
2008年 | 2152篇 |
2007年 | 2114篇 |
2006年 | 1983篇 |
2005年 | 1691篇 |
2004年 | 1703篇 |
2003年 | 1671篇 |
2002年 | 1579篇 |
2001年 | 2165篇 |
2000年 | 2021篇 |
1999年 | 1544篇 |
1998年 | 806篇 |
1997年 | 727篇 |
1996年 | 767篇 |
1995年 | 704篇 |
1994年 | 691篇 |
1993年 | 662篇 |
1992年 | 1234篇 |
1991年 | 1156篇 |
1990年 | 1114篇 |
1989年 | 1066篇 |
1988年 | 1016篇 |
1987年 | 966篇 |
1986年 | 976篇 |
1985年 | 1050篇 |
1984年 | 880篇 |
1983年 | 760篇 |
1982年 | 602篇 |
1981年 | 590篇 |
1979年 | 846篇 |
1978年 | 668篇 |
1977年 | 571篇 |
1976年 | 566篇 |
1975年 | 701篇 |
1974年 | 781篇 |
1973年 | 783篇 |
1972年 | 663篇 |
1971年 | 609篇 |
1968年 | 640篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
142.
143.
144.
M Tomonaga T Matsuzawa 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1992,106(1):43-52
In a conditional-discrimination task (matching-to-sample), we assessed similarities among figures consisting of 2 elemental figures through the choice reaction time, nonmetric multidimensional scaling, and hierarchical cluster analysis data from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens). Humans also rated similarities among figures. The results of the 3 experiments clearly indicated that the reaction time data obtained from chimpanzees' performances were useful measures of the similarities among figures. The results suggested that chimpanzees and humans perceived the complex figures similarly. The outer-contour elements were perceived most dominantly by both species, and the straight-line elements were perceived least dominantly. Both species showed the same perceptual hierarchy or dominance among perceptual categories, as determined by the similarity of simple elements, on the basis of transformational invariances. 相似文献
145.
146.
Two experiments are reported which investigated how subject gender and time-of-day influenced the estimation of duration and
the perception of task-related mental workload. In the first experiment, 24 subjects performed a filled time-estimation task
in a constant blacked-out, noise-reduced environment at 0800h, 1200h, 1600h, and 2000h, respectively. In the second experiment,
12 different subjects performed an unfilled time estimation task in similar conditions at 0900h, 1400h, and 1900h. At the
termination of all experimental sessions, participants completed the NASA Task Load Index workload assessment questionnaire
as a measure of perceived mental workload. Results indicated that physiological response, reflected in body temperature change,
followed an expected pattern of sequential increase with time-of-day. However, estimates of duration and the perception of
mental workload showed no significant effects for time-of-day. In each of the experiments there were significant differences
in time estimation and mental workload response contingent on the gender of the participant. These results are interpreted
in light of the previous positive findings for circadian fluctuation in performance efficiency and the equivocal findings
of a gender difference in time estimation. A unifying account of these collective results is given based on gender by time-of-day
interactional effects.
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 32nd Annual Meeting of the Human Factors Society, Anaheim, CA, October,
1988. 相似文献
147.
Royda Crose Donald R. Nicholas David C. Gobble Beth Frank 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1992,71(2):149-156
An expanded view of health is advocated for assessment and treatment of individual clients in helping them achieve and maintain their maximum potential for high-level wellness. In addition, moderating variables, such as culture, age, and gender differences, have an impact on every dimension of health and wellness. In this article a multidimensional systems model for wellness is described, and gender differences along those dimensions are reviewed. Application of the model and implications for counseling are discussed. Finally, two illustrative case examples, one man and one woman, are described. 相似文献
148.
The dimensions by which adults of differing ages experience emotion were studied by self-administering questionnaires administered to older adults (n = 828) recruited from Elderhostel programs, middle-aged (ages 30-59) children of Elderhostel attenders (n = 231), and young adult (ages 18-29) subjects recruited from college classes or through Elderhostel participants (n = 207). Elders were higher in emotional control, mood stability, and emotional maturity through moderation and leveling of positive affect and lower in surgency, psychophysiological responsiveness, and sensation seeking. These findings are consistent with the hypothesized increase in self-regulatory capacity with age. These cross-sectional differences cannot, however, be distinguished from cohort-related explanations; they require considerable replication across different types of subjects and further characterization of the dimensions in terms of their functions for self-regulation. 相似文献
149.
150.