首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   70125篇
  免费   1212篇
  国内免费   5篇
  71342篇
  2020年   875篇
  2019年   1049篇
  2018年   1441篇
  2017年   1439篇
  2016年   1554篇
  2015年   1109篇
  2014年   1364篇
  2013年   6209篇
  2012年   2444篇
  2011年   2485篇
  2010年   1533篇
  2009年   1554篇
  2008年   2152篇
  2007年   2114篇
  2006年   1983篇
  2005年   1691篇
  2004年   1703篇
  2003年   1671篇
  2002年   1579篇
  2001年   2165篇
  2000年   2021篇
  1999年   1544篇
  1998年   806篇
  1997年   727篇
  1996年   767篇
  1995年   704篇
  1994年   691篇
  1993年   662篇
  1992年   1234篇
  1991年   1156篇
  1990年   1114篇
  1989年   1066篇
  1988年   1016篇
  1987年   966篇
  1986年   976篇
  1985年   1050篇
  1984年   880篇
  1983年   760篇
  1982年   602篇
  1981年   590篇
  1979年   846篇
  1978年   668篇
  1977年   571篇
  1976年   566篇
  1975年   701篇
  1974年   781篇
  1973年   783篇
  1972年   663篇
  1971年   609篇
  1968年   640篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
Book review     
Jose M. Arcaya Ph.D. 《Group》1992,16(2):125-128
  相似文献   
142.
143.
144.
In a conditional-discrimination task (matching-to-sample), we assessed similarities among figures consisting of 2 elemental figures through the choice reaction time, nonmetric multidimensional scaling, and hierarchical cluster analysis data from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens). Humans also rated similarities among figures. The results of the 3 experiments clearly indicated that the reaction time data obtained from chimpanzees' performances were useful measures of the similarities among figures. The results suggested that chimpanzees and humans perceived the complex figures similarly. The outer-contour elements were perceived most dominantly by both species, and the straight-line elements were perceived least dominantly. Both species showed the same perceptual hierarchy or dominance among perceptual categories, as determined by the similarity of simple elements, on the basis of transformational invariances.  相似文献   
145.
146.
Two experiments are reported which investigated how subject gender and time-of-day influenced the estimation of duration and the perception of task-related mental workload. In the first experiment, 24 subjects performed a filled time-estimation task in a constant blacked-out, noise-reduced environment at 0800h, 1200h, 1600h, and 2000h, respectively. In the second experiment, 12 different subjects performed an unfilled time estimation task in similar conditions at 0900h, 1400h, and 1900h. At the termination of all experimental sessions, participants completed the NASA Task Load Index workload assessment questionnaire as a measure of perceived mental workload. Results indicated that physiological response, reflected in body temperature change, followed an expected pattern of sequential increase with time-of-day. However, estimates of duration and the perception of mental workload showed no significant effects for time-of-day. In each of the experiments there were significant differences in time estimation and mental workload response contingent on the gender of the participant. These results are interpreted in light of the previous positive findings for circadian fluctuation in performance efficiency and the equivocal findings of a gender difference in time estimation. A unifying account of these collective results is given based on gender by time-of-day interactional effects. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 32nd Annual Meeting of the Human Factors Society, Anaheim, CA, October, 1988.  相似文献   
147.
An expanded view of health is advocated for assessment and treatment of individual clients in helping them achieve and maintain their maximum potential for high-level wellness. In addition, moderating variables, such as culture, age, and gender differences, have an impact on every dimension of health and wellness. In this article a multidimensional systems model for wellness is described, and gender differences along those dimensions are reviewed. Application of the model and implications for counseling are discussed. Finally, two illustrative case examples, one man and one woman, are described.  相似文献   
148.
The dimensions by which adults of differing ages experience emotion were studied by self-administering questionnaires administered to older adults (n = 828) recruited from Elderhostel programs, middle-aged (ages 30-59) children of Elderhostel attenders (n = 231), and young adult (ages 18-29) subjects recruited from college classes or through Elderhostel participants (n = 207). Elders were higher in emotional control, mood stability, and emotional maturity through moderation and leveling of positive affect and lower in surgency, psychophysiological responsiveness, and sensation seeking. These findings are consistent with the hypothesized increase in self-regulatory capacity with age. These cross-sectional differences cannot, however, be distinguished from cohort-related explanations; they require considerable replication across different types of subjects and further characterization of the dimensions in terms of their functions for self-regulation.  相似文献   
149.
150.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号