全文获取类型
收费全文 | 136篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
136篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 2篇 |
2016年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 4篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 5篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 2篇 |
2007年 | 2篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
1963年 | 1篇 |
1961年 | 2篇 |
1959年 | 3篇 |
1958年 | 4篇 |
1957年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
1955年 | 4篇 |
1954年 | 3篇 |
1952年 | 6篇 |
1951年 | 2篇 |
1950年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有136条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
Previous studies suggest that adults and children divide spoken syllables into subsyllabic onset-rime units more readily than into any other kind of subsyllabic unit. We asked whether this same onset-rime segmentation might also be beneficial in teaching children to read. That is, can children learn more words segmented at the onset-rime boundary (e.g., CL-AP, D-ISH) than words segmented after the vowel (CLA-P, DI-SH)? In three experiments, first-grade students studied single words presented by a computer connected to a high-quality speech synthesizer. Experiment 1 used words of four letters but only three phonemes apiece (e.g., WHIP, DISH). In some of these words the onset-rime segmentation corresponded to the initial bigram (e.g., WH-IP); in some it did not (e.g., D-ISH). Experiments 2 and 3 used words of four letters and four phonemes (e.g., CLAP, CORN). In all three experiments, onset-rime segmentation proved more helpful than postvowel segmentation in short-term learning of the words. 相似文献
22.
The usual superiority in frequency-of-occurrence judgments of younger vs. older subjects was hypothesized to result from greater strategic encoding of the materials conveying frequency information. A subject-paced, visual search task was designed to control nontarget word encoding. Relative frequency judgments for the nontarget word pairs were equally accurate for younger and older subjects, and performance of both groups was above chance. Results suggest that strategic cover-task encoding can induce age differences in incidental frequency processing. Consistent with a nonoptimal as opposed to an optimal view, automatic processes require only minimal capacity for above-chance performance, but additional strategic resources can increase performance. When such additional resources are used more by younger than by older subjects, the former are better in frequency performance. 相似文献
23.
Edward A. Wise 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》1994,1(2):167-181
The MBHI and MMPI personality disorder scales were analyzed for convergent and discriminant validity. Correlational data demonstrated that six of the eight scales were significantly related, while the remaining two scales approached significance. Further analyses of these data, however, demonstrated that none of the scales correlated significantly better with its convergent scale compared to nonconvergent scales. The MBHI classified significantly more of the sample as personality disordered (93%) compared to the MMPI personality disorder scales (17%). Furthermore, the MBHI tended to describe the sample as falling within the Anxious cluster of personality disorders, whereas the MMPI described them within the Dramatic cluster. Single scale codetype correspondence was found to be 15%, while two-point concordance was 12.5%, indicating very low congruence between personality style codetypes. These two measures do not appear to be measuring the same personality style constructs. 相似文献
24.
25.
This article is concerned with how information about time and position in a sequence is represented in short-term memory and
expressed in the dynamics of serial recall. Temporal-distinctiveness theories of memory predict that isolating a list item
in time will improve recall accuracy for that item. Although the majority of research in short-term memory has failed to demonstrate
a temporal isolation effect (TIE), there are occasions on which a TIE is observed. The disparity in results has been explained
by assuming that participants can adaptively weight temporal and nontemporal information at retrieval, with differences between
experiments promoting or discouraging reliance on time as a source of episodic information. A particular focus of the present
study is the finding that the TIE is substantially observed in standard serial recall only when participants are instructed
to group the list into minisequences. The findings of two experiments using instructed grouping replicated this effect but
showed that it is attributable to a longer gap at the group boundary enhancing the positive effect of grouping on recall accuracy.
These results show that the hierarchical representations usually associated with temporal grouping are also elicited by instructed
grouping but that an additional and nonspecific benefit to recall obtains from lengthening the pause between groups. An additional
role for time is identified in the timing of responses: The dynamics of input sequences tend to be mirrored in output sequences
for ungrouped lists, whereas the primacy pattern in grouped lists is for a longer duration to speed access to the following
group when that duration occurs at an instructed group boundary. 相似文献
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.