首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   587篇
  免费   60篇
  国内免费   79篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   15篇
  2020年   21篇
  2019年   18篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   25篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   25篇
  2013年   55篇
  2012年   26篇
  2011年   38篇
  2010年   21篇
  2009年   37篇
  2008年   36篇
  2007年   44篇
  2006年   26篇
  2005年   35篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   8篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   5篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   6篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   4篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   4篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   3篇
排序方式: 共有726条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
631.
From a basis in social exchange theory, the authors investigated whether, and how, negative feedback-seeking behavior and a team empowerment climate affect the relationship between leader-member exchange (LMX) and member performance. Results showed that subordinates' negative feedback-seeking behavior mediated the relationship between LMX and both objective and subjective in-role performance. In addition, the level of a team's empowerment climate was positively related to subordinates' own sense of empowerment, which in turn negatively moderated the effects of LMX on negative feedback-seeking behavior.  相似文献   
632.
The current study aims to separate conscious and unconscious behaviors by employing both online and offline measures while the participants were consciously performing a task. Using an eye-movement tracking paradigm, we observed participants' response patterns for distinguishing within-word-boundary and across-word-boundary reverse errors while reading Chinese sentences (also known as the "word inferiority effect"). The results showed that when the participants consciously detected errors, their gaze time for target words associated with across-word-boundary reverse errors was significantly longer than that for targets words associated with within-word-boundary reverse errors. Surprisingly, the same gaze time pattern was found even when the readers were not consciously aware of the reverse errors. The results were statistically robust, providing converging evidence for the feasibility of our experimental paradigm in decoupling offline behaviors and the online, automatic, and unconscious aspects of cognitive processing in reading.  相似文献   
633.
Timing and trajectory in rhythm production   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
The Wing-Kristofferson movement timing model (A. M. Wing & A. B. Kristofferson, 1973a, 1973b) distinguishes central timer and motor implementation processes. Previous studies have shown that increases in interresponse interval (IRI) variability with mean IRI are due to central timer processes, not motor implementation. The authors examine whether this is true with IRI duration changes in binary rhythm production. Ten participants provided IRI and movement data in bimanual synchronous tapping under equal (isochronous) and alternating (rhythm) interval conditions. Movement trajectory changes were observed with IRI duration (300, 500, or 833 ms) and for 500-ms IRIs produced in rhythm contexts (300/500 ms, 500/833 ms). However, application of the Wing-Kristofferson model showed that duration and context effects on IRI variability were attributable largely to timer processes with relatively little effect on motor processes.  相似文献   
634.
虚拟团队学习效能研究:社会认知因素的影响   总被引:11,自引:0,他引:11  
唐宁玉  王重鸣 《心理科学》2007,30(1):227-231,215
本研究采用现场研究的方法,对21个虚拟团队学习的效能进行了实证研究。研究对社会认知因素中的效能感和目标取向对虚拟团队学习效能(技能改变、工作任务和团队健康水平)的影响进行了研究,并探讨了对虚拟团队的态度在中间所起的作用。研究发现,虚拟团队效能感和学习取向对虚拟团队的学习效能产生作用,而对虚拟团队学习的态度、团队信任对虚拟团队学习的效能起着中介或直接的作用。  相似文献   
635.
采取DRM范式,要求被试判断词表项目的字形结构或音调,从而间接学习词表,考察了关联性、不同编码方式和测验情境(测验次数、测验词颜色两个变量)对真实和错误记忆的影响。实验发现:(1)随着关联性的增加,未学词的错误再认率也增加,说明关联性是引发错误记忆的必要条件;(2)字形判断比字音判断下的错误再认率要高,说明汉字中形—义联结可能比音—义联结更加紧密;(3)测验情境对真实和错误记忆影响并不相同,表明两者虽然同源,但具有完全不同的机制。  相似文献   
636.
We present a tutorial on a power spectral approach to variability in serial motor performance, describing as a case study two experiments on the form of the variance in two force production tasks. In Experiment 1 we examine grip force and load force in repetitive unimanual pulling; in Experiment 2, we describe repetitive bimanual pressing. In both experiments log-log plots of power spectral density of peak force of the responses in each stream against frequency (i.e. periodicity or repetition cycle defined with respect to the ordered succession of responses) were approximately linear with negative slopes which varied systematically with test conditions. We propose the two response streams in each experiment are associated with different levels of sustained attention and state a simple model involving summation of moving average processes on multiple time scales as a qualitative account of the changes in 1/f slope.  相似文献   
637.
孔子的经济伦理思想研究   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
孔子的经济伦理思想构建是以“义利之辨”为基础 ,以“富国安民”为中心 ,以“均而安”和“节用”等德政措施为手段 ,达到经济发展与社会稳定的双向目的 ,具有德性主义的性质。  相似文献   
638.
Frames of reference in mobile augmented reality displays   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
In 3 experiments, the authors investigated spatial updating in augmented reality environments. Participants learned locations of virtual objects on the physical floor. They were turned to appropriate facing directions while blindfolded before making pointing judgments (e.g., "Imagine you are facing X. Point to Y"). Experiments manipulated the angular difference between the learning heading and the imagined heading and between the actual heading and the imagined heading. The effect of actual-imagined on pointing latency was observed for naive users but not for users with brief training or instructions concerning the fact that objects can move with body movements. The results indicated that naive users used an environment-stabilized reference frame to access information arrays, but with experience and instruction the nature of the representation changed from an environment stabilized to a body stabilized reference frame.  相似文献   
639.
Sign languages modulate the production of signs in space and use this spatial modulation to refer back to entities—to maintain coreference. We ask here whether spatial modulation is so fundamental to language in the manual modality that it will be invented by individuals asked to create gestures on the spot. English speakers were asked to describe vignettes under 2 conditions: using gesture without speech, and using speech with spontaneous gestures. When using gesture alone, adults placed gestures for particular entities in non-neutral locations and then used those locations to refer back to the entities. When using gesture plus speech, adults also produced gestures in non-neutral locations but used the locations coreferentially far less often. When gesture is forced to take on the full burden of communication, it exploits space for coreference. Coreference thus appears to be a resilient property of language, likely to emerge in communication systems no matter how simple.  相似文献   
640.
Outside the laboratory, rats (Rattus norvegicus) are likely both to interact with several conspecifics that have eaten various foods and to eat a variety of foods themselves before they encounter any particular food for which they have a socially enhanced preference. Here the authors examine the stability of rats' socially learned food preferences following 6 days of potentially disruptive ingestive experiences. The authors found that 6 days of (a) eating unfamiliar foods, (b) interacting with demonstrators that had eaten unfamiliar foods, or (c) both eating unfamiliar foods and interacting with demonstrators that had eaten those foods had no measurable effect on rats' socially learned food preferences. The stability of socially enhanced food preferences over time and despite potentially disruptive experiences is consistent with the view that social learning about foods is an important determinant of the food choices of free-living Norway rats.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号