首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   18854篇
  免费   902篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2021年   155篇
  2020年   256篇
  2019年   294篇
  2018年   412篇
  2017年   418篇
  2016年   438篇
  2015年   323篇
  2014年   350篇
  2013年   1541篇
  2012年   641篇
  2011年   722篇
  2010年   432篇
  2009年   418篇
  2008年   537篇
  2007年   504篇
  2006年   450篇
  2005年   386篇
  2004年   396篇
  2003年   386篇
  2002年   380篇
  2001年   730篇
  2000年   730篇
  1999年   481篇
  1998年   198篇
  1997年   193篇
  1996年   160篇
  1993年   153篇
  1992年   385篇
  1991年   346篇
  1990年   374篇
  1989年   350篇
  1988年   347篇
  1987年   309篇
  1986年   326篇
  1985年   330篇
  1984年   253篇
  1983年   243篇
  1981年   156篇
  1979年   214篇
  1978年   174篇
  1975年   207篇
  1974年   255篇
  1973年   221篇
  1972年   214篇
  1971年   177篇
  1970年   154篇
  1969年   208篇
  1968年   220篇
  1967年   180篇
  1966年   193篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 234 毫秒
221.
222.
223.
Hypnotic hallucination alters evoked potentials   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
  相似文献   
224.
225.
226.
Development of processing speed was examined in three backward masking studies. The first verified the central nature of backward masking for children aged 8 and 11 years and for adults. The second suggested that task requirements were equivalent for children similar to those in Study 1, and that age differences in performance were not attributable to nonprocessing variables. The main cross-sequential study estimated speed of processing in 80 children (approximately 6 years to 13 years) and young adults using an inspection time task. Target exposure duration was varied to establish the time required to achieve a high level of discriminative accuracy. Estimates of processing speed increased until about 11-13 years of age; beyond this, the trend was less obvious, and it is possible that inspection time asymptotes at around the onset of adolescence. Performance improvement after 1 year could not be explained as resulting from practice since improvement among controls over a period of 2 weeks was significantly less. Correlations between estimates of inspection time made up to 2 years apart found the measure to be reliable.  相似文献   
227.
Two experiments assessed the importance of sound duration for eliciting head orientation responses from newborn infants. In Experiment 1, thirty infants turned with equal frequency toward 20-s continuous rattle sounds and 20-s trains of rattle segments. The duration of the rattle segments--14 and 100 ms (2/s), or 500 ms (1/s)--did not influence the likelihood of turning. Response latencies and durations proved quite similar for all stimuli. In Experiment 2, twenty-four infants heard continuous rattle sounds of four different durations: 1, 5, 10, and 20 s. They turned reliably to all stimulus durations; furthermore, the magnitude and temporal characteristics of head orientation responses did not differ for the four stimulus durations. These results suggest that the newborn's head orientation response may reflect a motor program that is initiated by auditory input and then executed in a similar fashion regardless of further stimulation.  相似文献   
228.
Sex roles, gender, and fear   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Male and female college students classified as masculine, feminine, androgynous, or undifferentiated on the Bem (1974) Sex Role Inventory (N = 199) were given the Wolpe (1969) Fear Inventory. Significant differences in average fear scores were seen between men and women and between the four sex-role categories, with sex role and gender contributing equally to the variability. The argument is raised here that excessive fearfulness or fearlessness might be as much a product of sex-role conditioning as a product of gender.  相似文献   
229.
230.
When discriminated interresponse-time (IRT) procedures have been used to assess preference relations among temporally extended operants, deviations from matching have been obtained. Using a yoked-control procedure, the present study found that key pecking in a discriminated IRT procedure has two sources of strength--that arising from the response-reinforcer contingency that is explicitly arranged, and that arising from a stimulus-reinforcer contingency that is a by-product of the explicitly arranged contingency. The key pecking of all lead birds, and that of 3 of the 4 birds exposed to a yoked autoshaping procedure, was controlled by the keylight that signaled the lead birds' criterion IRTs. Because stimulus control of key pecking by the keylight, whether autoshaped or discriminative, fosters deviations from matching, the discriminated IRT procedure does not provide an appropriate basis for conclusions about preference relations among IRTs.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号