全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1062篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
1082篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 27篇 |
2013年 | 105篇 |
2012年 | 39篇 |
2011年 | 47篇 |
2010年 | 35篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 42篇 |
2007年 | 29篇 |
2006年 | 35篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 35篇 |
2001年 | 30篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 29篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有1082条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
The journal articles in the extant philosophical literature which argue in favor of carrying handguns for self-defense tend to assume that these weapons will be concealed and make no mention of carrying them openly. This paper aims to show that, since open-carry can be more effective for self-defense than concealed-carry, any argument for a moral right to carry a handgun for self-defense which relies on a claim of their effectiveness and which assumes concealed-carry, entails the moral right to carry them openly in public.
相似文献22.
23.
24.
Slade?J.?RiegerEmail author Tracey?Peter Lance?W.?Roberts 《Journal of religion and health》2015,54(6):2005-2019
Scholarly research focusing on social psychological factors (e.g. mental health) and social environmental factors (e.g. childhood trauma) has found these measures to be correlated with suicidality. However, such literature has tended to overlook what may impact one’s reasons for living. Using a sample of over 1,200 students from a Canadian university, the goal of the current study is to empirically test, by employing multivariate nested regression models (by levels of suicidal behaviour), known and relative unknown correlates with reasons for living, with a particular focus on strength of religious faith, which is a well-known predictor for suicidality, but less studied as a reason for living. Results show that, among students with serious suicidal ideation and/or a previous suicide attempt, the strongest predictor for student’s reasons for living was strength of religious faith. Strength of religious faith has seldom been acknowledged or identified as an important measure in assessing one’s reasons to live. These findings have implications for the role of religiosity among suicidality research, especially studies that focus on reasons for living. 相似文献
25.
26.
Although there has been a proliferation of models of supervision in the marriage and family therapy literature recently, most tend to focus on methods rather than on the process of supervision. The model presented here is grounded in developmental concepts and focuses on student learning through a dialectical process of cognitive and emotional growth and incorporates three stages: 1) developing relationships; 2) breaking impasses, and 3) orchestrating changes. Students discover that supervision becomes isomorphic with relationship issues that arise in their therapy sessions. Dialectical in nature in that each stage is characterized by the resolution of contradictory emotions, the supervision process helps supervisees experience emotional shifts in their interactions with others, referred to as emotional restructuring. 相似文献
27.
On the basis of a conditioning analysis of the orientation-contingent color aftereffect (McCollough effect, ME), orientation stimuli become associated with simultaneously presented chromatic stimuli. This account suggests that decreasing the contingency between the grid orientation and color should decrease the strength of the aftereffect. Results of previous research indicate that decreasing the temporal contingency (by presenting homogeneous chromatic stimuli between presentations of chromatic grids) does not decrease the ME. However, it has been suggested that the appropriate contingency-degradation procedure would involve decreasing spatial (rather than temporal) contingency. That is, the illusion should be attenuated by extending the color beyond the confines of the grid. Contrary to this hypothesis, the results of the present experiments provide no evidence that decreasing the spatial contingency between grid and color decreases the ME; rather, the aftereffect is increased by such a manipulation. 相似文献
28.
Olthof A Roberts WA 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2000,114(2):158-166
In each of 3 experiments, different sets of 4 pigeons (Columba livia) were trained to discriminate between 2 visual symbols that covered wells containing food items that varied in number, mass, or both. In Experiment 1, the symbols were associated with 0, 1, 3, 5, 7, or 9 pieces of grain reward. The pigeons learned to choose the symbol corresponding to the larger reward, and on summation tests, they chose the pair of symbols that summed to the larger total reward. When number of food pellets was varied but mass of reward was held constant in Experiment 2, preference for the larger number symbols failed to appear. When number was held constant and mass was varied in Experiment 3, the pigeons showed a clear preference for the larger mass symbols on single-symbol and summation tests. These findings show that pigeons summate the value of symbols and are more likely to represent symbols by mass of food reward than by number of food items. 相似文献
29.
AbstractEarly aiming adjustments following an online perturbation are made possible by impulse control. This process may unfold even earlier when perturbations impose a greater risk of a costly overshoot error. Participants executed upward and downward aims to mediate the cost of potential errors—downward overshoots require more energy to correct against gravity. On 33% of the trials, texture elements on the aiming surface were shifted following onset to appear congruent or incongruent with the aiming direction, and consequently generate a misperception of the limb moving slower or faster, respectively. Thus, the risk of potential errors could be influenced by the online perturbation (e.g., increased perceived likelihood of overshooting following the incongruent background). Findings indicated greater undershooting for down compared to up, which reflects the principle of movement optimisation. There was also more undershooting for an incongruent compared to congruent background, which is consistent with early online adjustments counter-acting the misperceived limb velocity. However, there were no interactions throughout the movement trajectory. We suggest that while the initial pre-programme considers the cost of potential errors (target direction), early impulse control fails to discriminate the likelihood of these errors occurring following an online perturbation (moving background). 相似文献
30.