全文获取类型
收费全文 | 38977篇 |
免费 | 729篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
39708篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 509篇 |
2018年 | 643篇 |
2017年 | 670篇 |
2016年 | 682篇 |
2015年 | 500篇 |
2014年 | 643篇 |
2013年 | 2932篇 |
2012年 | 1117篇 |
2011年 | 1223篇 |
2010年 | 727篇 |
2009年 | 744篇 |
2008年 | 1044篇 |
2007年 | 1053篇 |
2006年 | 970篇 |
2005年 | 881篇 |
2004年 | 861篇 |
2003年 | 811篇 |
2002年 | 850篇 |
2001年 | 1092篇 |
2000年 | 994篇 |
1999年 | 786篇 |
1998年 | 468篇 |
1997年 | 415篇 |
1996年 | 382篇 |
1992年 | 638篇 |
1991年 | 605篇 |
1990年 | 590篇 |
1989年 | 609篇 |
1988年 | 595篇 |
1987年 | 564篇 |
1986年 | 573篇 |
1985年 | 625篇 |
1984年 | 528篇 |
1983年 | 507篇 |
1982年 | 447篇 |
1981年 | 425篇 |
1979年 | 598篇 |
1978年 | 472篇 |
1977年 | 405篇 |
1976年 | 433篇 |
1975年 | 532篇 |
1974年 | 556篇 |
1973年 | 562篇 |
1972年 | 458篇 |
1971年 | 436篇 |
1970年 | 420篇 |
1969年 | 433篇 |
1968年 | 523篇 |
1967年 | 447篇 |
1966年 | 453篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
We explored college students' discrimination of complex visual stimuli that involvedmultiple-item displays. The items in each of the displays could be all the same, all different, or diverse mixtures of some same and some different items. The participants had to learn which of two arbitrary responses was correct for each of the displays without being told about the sameness or differentness of the stimuli. We observed a general improvement in discrimination performance--a rise in choice accuracy and a fall in reaction time-as the number of icons in the display was increased, even when the participants had been trained from the outset with displays containing different numbers of items and when smaller numbers of items were not randomly distributed but grouped in the center of the display. The participants' discrimination behavior also depended on the mixture of same and different items in the displays. Striking individual differences in the participants' discrimination behavior disclosed that people sometimes respond as do pigeons and baboons trained with a similar task. This and previous related research suggest that variability discrimination may lie at the root of same-different categorization behavior. 相似文献
962.
E. J. LOWE 《Philosophy and phenomenological research》2006,72(2):466-479
John Heil's new book (Heil 2003) is remarkable in many ways. In a concise, lucid and accessible manner, it develops a complete system of ontology with many strikingly original features and then applies that ontology to fundamental issues in the philosophy of mind, with illuminating results. Although Heil acknowledges his intellectual debts to C. B. Martin (p. viii), he is unduly modest about his own contribution to the development and application of this novel metaphysical system. A full examination of the position that Heil defends would require a book in itself, so I shall limit myself to discussing some of its central themes and to explaining a few doubts that I have concerning some aspects of it. 相似文献
963.
We tested the hypothesis that a psychosocial dental intervention formulated in terms of self-determination theory would increase
patients’ perceived competence and autonomous motivation for dental care and would decrease their plaque and gingivitis over
a seven month period, compared to standard dental treatment. We also tested a process model in which the intervention was
expected to increase perceived dental competence and autonomous motivation, that they would be positively associated with
oral health behaviors (i.e., brushing and flossing), which was expected to decrease plaque and, in turn, decrease gingivitis.
We also examined whether: changes in perceived competence and autonomous motivation would mediate the effect of the intervention
on dental-health behaviors; dental-health behaviors would mediate the links from changes in perceived competence and autonomous
motivation to change in plaque; and change in plaque would mediate the relation of dental health behaviors to change in gingivitis.
Finally, we examined the fit of the overall model with structural equation modelling. Results supported all predictions.
相似文献
Anne E. Münster HalvariEmail: |
964.
The current investigations build upon previous ethnographic research, which identified a social norm for adolescent females to engage in “fat talk” (informal dialogue during which individuals express body dissatisfaction). In Study 1, participants were shown a vignette involving women engaging in fat talk dialogue and were subsequently asked to chose one of three self-presentational responses for a target female: (1) self-accepting of her body, (2) providing no information, or (3) self-degrading about her body. Male and female participants believed the target would be most likely to self-degrade, and that this would lead women to like her, while the self-accepting response would lead men to like her most. Study 2 used the same vignette but participants were asked to respond in an open-ended fashion. Participants again expected the target female to self-degrade. The present findings suggest college students perceive fat talk self-degradation of body image as normative. 相似文献
965.
A Longitudinal Study of Relational Aggression, Physical Aggression, and Children's Social–Psychological Adjustment 总被引:1,自引:2,他引:1
Nicki R. Crick Jamie M. Ostrov Nicole E. Werner 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2006,34(2):127-138
Although great strides have recently been made in our understanding of relational aggression and its consequences, one significant limitation has been the lack of prospective studies. The present research addressed this issue by identifying and assessing groups of relationally aggressive, physically aggressive, relationally plus physically aggressive (co-morbid), and nonaggressive children during their third grade year in elementary school and then reassessing them a year later, during fourth-grade (N = 224, 113 girls). Two aspects of social–psychological adjustment were assessed during both assessment periods including internalizing difficulties (i.e., withdrawal, depression/anxiety, and somatic complaints) and externalizing problems (i.e., aggressive behavior, delinquency). It was revealed that the strongest predictor of future social–psychological adjustment problems and increases in these problems from third to fourth was the combination of relational and physical aggression. Relational aggression also contributed unique information, relative to physical aggression, in the prediction of future maladjustment. Implications of these findings for future research and prevention efforts, particularly for aggressive girls, are discussed.
相似文献
Nicki R. CrickEmail: |
966.
967.
Norman JF Crabtree CE Herrmann M Thompson SR Shular CF Clayton AM 《Perception & psychophysics》2006,68(1):94-101
In two experiments, we investigated the ability of younger and older observers to perceive and discriminate 3-D shape from static and dynamic patterns of binocular disparity. In both experiments, the younger observers' discrimination accuracies were 20% higher than those of the older observers. Despite this quantitative difference, in all other respects the older observers performed similarly to the younger observers. Both age groups were similarly affected by changes in the magnitude of binocular disparity, by reductions in binocular correspondence, and by increases in the speed of stereoscopic motion. In addition, observers in both age groups exhibited an advantage in performance for dynamic stereograms when the patterns of binocular disparity contained significant amounts of correspondence "noise." The process of aging does affect stereopsis, but the effects are quantitative rather than qualitative. 相似文献
968.
Bruce AE Cole DA Dallaire DH Jacquez FM Pineda AQ LaGrange B 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2006,34(3):310-322
In a sample of 299 children (grades 2, 4, and 6), we examined parenting and negative life events as predictors of depressive cognitions, specifically low self-perceived competence, depressive cognitive schemas, and depressogenic attributional style. We also examined developmental trends in these relations. Children completed measures of parenting, negative life events, and depressive cognitions. Parents also completed measures of parenting and negative life events. Consistent with our hypotheses, negative parenting and negative life events corresponded with higher levels of depressive cognitions, whereas positive parenting corresponded with lower levels of depressive cognitions. The relations between negative parenting and negative automatic thoughts were stronger for older children. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
969.
Dishabituation in Aplysia can involve either reversal of habituation or superimposed sensitization 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
下载免费PDF全文

Dishabituation has been thought to be due either to reversal of the process of habituation or to a second process equivalent to sensitization superimposed on habituation. One way to address this question is by testing whether dishabituation and sensitization can be dissociated. Previous studies using this approach in Aplysia have come to different conclusions about the nature of dishabituation, perhaps because those studies differed in many respects, including (1) whether they also observed transient behavioral inhibition, and (2) whether they used test stimuli that activated the LE siphon sensory neurons or as yet unidentified sensory neurons. To attempt to resolve the apparent contradictions between the previous studies, we have explored the importance of these two factors by performing a parametric study of dishabituation and sensitization of gill withdrawal in a simplified preparation that does not exhibit transient behavioral inhibition, using two different test stimuli that are known to activate the LE (Touch) or unidentified (Not Touch) sensory neurons. We find that dishabituation and sensitization in this preparation have similar time courses and generally similar functions of shock intensity. However, under one condition, with the Not Touch stimulus 2.5 min after the shock, dishabituation has a reverse effect of shock intensity. Additional analyses suggest that dishabituation with the Not Touch stimulus 2.5 min after the shock is due to reversal of habituation, whereas 12.5 min after the shock, dishabituation is due to superimposed sensitization. These results thus suggest that dishabituation may involve either process in the same preparation, and begin to define the conditions that favor one or the other. 相似文献
970.
Individuals differ in the number of corpus callosum (CC) nerve fibers interconnecting their cerebral hemispheres by about threefold. Early reports suggested that males had smaller CCs than females. This was often interpreted to support the concept that the male brain is more "lateralized" or "specialized," thus accounting for presumed male predominance in mathematics, as well as for aggressive behavior. Ultimately, meta-analyses of these many reports found no significant overall sex differences in inter-cerebral information carrying capacity. Here, using quantitative MRI, we found the midline CC area of 113 subjects was significantly correlated, not with handedness or sex, but with dichotic deafness, and even more so with redefined hemisphericity, the latter accounting for over 19% of CC variability. That is, both dichotic hearing and right brain-oriented individuals of either sex had significantly larger CCs than dichotically deaf or left brain-oriented persons. Thus, current traditions of brain laterality and gender may benefit from revisions that include redefined hemisphericity. 相似文献