全文获取类型
收费全文 | 36669篇 |
免费 | 1534篇 |
国内免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
38230篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 407篇 |
2019年 | 505篇 |
2018年 | 706篇 |
2017年 | 656篇 |
2016年 | 746篇 |
2015年 | 507篇 |
2014年 | 635篇 |
2013年 | 3011篇 |
2012年 | 1144篇 |
2011年 | 1185篇 |
2010年 | 706篇 |
2009年 | 792篇 |
2008年 | 990篇 |
2007年 | 1040篇 |
2006年 | 909篇 |
2005年 | 811篇 |
2004年 | 797篇 |
2003年 | 745篇 |
2002年 | 787篇 |
2001年 | 1127篇 |
2000年 | 1127篇 |
1999年 | 850篇 |
1998年 | 418篇 |
1997年 | 400篇 |
1995年 | 388篇 |
1993年 | 385篇 |
1992年 | 714篇 |
1991年 | 659篇 |
1990年 | 635篇 |
1989年 | 612篇 |
1988年 | 605篇 |
1987年 | 554篇 |
1986年 | 541篇 |
1985年 | 591篇 |
1984年 | 510篇 |
1983年 | 484篇 |
1982年 | 404篇 |
1979年 | 544篇 |
1978年 | 430篇 |
1977年 | 394篇 |
1976年 | 394篇 |
1975年 | 463篇 |
1974年 | 512篇 |
1973年 | 516篇 |
1972年 | 455篇 |
1971年 | 395篇 |
1970年 | 366篇 |
1968年 | 450篇 |
1967年 | 404篇 |
1966年 | 392篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Smoking, caffeine, and stress: effects on blood pressure and heart rate in male and female college students 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
This study examined the independent and joint effects of cigarette smoking and caffeine consumption on blood pressure (BP) and heart rate (HR) reactions to stress in male and female college students. Following an initial physiological baseline, participants received one of four experimental treatments (paced smoking, caffeine, smoking plus caffeine, or neither) and had BP and HR measured before, during, and after two stressful tasks. The results revealed that, compared with the control condition, caffeine ingestion enhanced the magnitude of stress-induced systolic BP and HR reactions. Smoking combined additively with stress, and the joint effect of smoking and caffeine was no greater than either taken alone. Males and females were generally similar in their BP and HR responses to smoking, caffeine, and stress. Inconsistencies with previous research and possible physiological mechanisms underlying the observed effects are discussed. 相似文献
102.
Two experiments investigated the effects of successive reinforcement contexts on choice. In the first, concurrent variable-interval schedules of primary reinforcement operated during the initial links of concurrent chains. The rate of this reinforcement arranged by the concurrent schedules was decreased across conditions: When it was higher than the terminal-link rate, preference for the higher frequency initial-link schedule increased relative to baseline. (During baseline, a standard concurrent-schedule procedure was in effect). When the initial-link reinforcement rate was lower than the terminal-link rate, preference converged toward indifference. In the second experiment, a chain schedule was available on a third key while a concurrent schedule was in effect on the side keys. When the terminal link of the chain schedule was produced, the side keys became inoperative. Availability of the chain schedule did not affect choice between the concurrent schedules. These results show that only when successive reinforcement contexts are produced by choice responding do those successive contexts affect choice in concurrent schedules. 相似文献
103.
Typical delay determines waiting time on periodic-food schedules: Static and dynamic tests 总被引:6,自引:6,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Pigeons and other animals soon learn to wait (pause) after food delivery on periodic-food schedules before resuming the food-rewarded response. Under most conditions the steady-state duration of the average waiting time, t, is a linear function of the typical interfood interval. We describe three experiments designed to explore the limits of this process. In all experiments, t was associated with one key color and the subsequent food delay, T, with another. In the first experiment, we compared the relation between t (waiting time) and T (food delay) under two conditions: when T was held constant, and when T was an inverse function of t. The pigeons could maximize the rate of food delivery under the first condition by setting t to a consistently short value; optimal behavior under the second condition required a linear relation with unit slope between t and T. Despite this difference in optimal policy, the pigeons in both cases showed the same linear relation, with slope less than one, between t and T. This result was confirmed in a second parametric experiment that added a third condition, in which T + t was held constant. Linear waiting appears to be an obligatory rule for pigeons. In a third experiment we arranged for a multiplicative relation between t and T (positive feedback), and produced either very short or very long waiting times as predicted by a quasi-dynamic model in which waiting time is strongly determined by the just-preceding food delay. 相似文献
104.
Jeffrey S. Levin Ph.D. M.P.H. C. David Jenkins Ph.D. Robert M. Rose M. D. 《Journal of religion and health》1988,27(4):267-278
In a study of air traffic controllers, religious differences are found in the way Type A behavior is associated with several health status indicators. Associations between the Jenkins Activity Survey (JAS) and physical illness incidence, health-promotive behavior, diastolic and systolic blood pressure, subjective distress and impulse control problems, and alcohol consumption are examined by religious attendance, religious affiliation, and change in affiliation. Findings confirm that Type A does not vary significantly by religion. However, there are several significant findings between Type A and various health indicators. Type A is associated with illness incidence, overall and more strongly in several religion, subgroups. Type A and alcohol consumption are related positively in Protestants and converts, and negatively in churchgoing Catholics. Type A is related to impulse control problems in churchgoing Protestants and to subjective distress in churchgoing Catholics. Finally, in individuals with weak or no religious ties, Type A is associated with lower blood pressure. This last finding suggests that in some people (for example, the irreligious or unchurched), the coronary-prone behavior pattern may have cardiovascular effects which are salutary in at least one respect.The authors wish to thank Dr. Kyriakos S. Markides and Laura A. Ray for their assistance with this study. Address requests for reprints to Dr. Levin at the Institute of Gerontology, 300 North Ingalls, Ann Arbor, MI 48109. 相似文献
105.
106.
Jeffrey C. King 《Philosophical Studies》1988,53(3):417-440
I have benefited greatly from discussing the issues addressed in this paper with Michael Liston, John Vickers and Mark Wilson. 相似文献
107.
The purpose of this study was to investigate differences in scores of 59 mentally retarded adults (mild, moderate, severe, or profound), 133 advantaged, and 130 disadvantaged (low income) children in Grades 1, 2, 3 on the Primary Measures of Music Audiation. Test-retest reliabilities for the Tonal and Rhythm subtests were .81 and .86, respectively, for the retarded group. Analysis of variance indicated that the mildly retarded children performed significantly better than other groups of retarded children on these two tests. 2 x 3 analyses of variance indicated that third graders from both advantaged and disadvantaged groups performed significantly better than the first or second graders on both subtests and that advantaged children performed significantly better than the disadvantaged. However, disadvantaged children made greater gains, especially for third graders, than the advantaged. 相似文献
108.
109.
110.
William Epstein 《Psychological research》1988,50(1):2-6
Summary The distinction is drawn between the metatheoretical commitments of Gestalt theory and their instantiations in explicit theory. It is argued that the metatheoretical commitments of Gestalt theory reflect important insights which might have served as the foundation of a theory of perception that avoids a number of the major difficulties associated with constructivism and the theory of direct perception. For this reason rehabilitation of the reputation of Gestalt theory is urged. In the concluding section certain similarities between the foundational premises of Gestalt theory and parallel distributed processing approaches are noted.The contents of this paper were presented initially at a symposium on perceptual theory in honor of Irvin Rock which convened in Washington, DC, on the occasion of the 1986 meetings of the American Psychological Association 相似文献