全文获取类型
收费全文 | 337篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
专业分类
374篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 17篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 10篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 34篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有374条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
62.
Two different concepts of distinguishability are often mixed up in attempts to derive in quantum mechanics the (anti)symmetry of the wave function from indistinguishability of identical particles. Some of these attempts are analyzed and shown to be defective. It is argued that, although identical particles should be considered as observationally indistinguishable in (anti)symmetric states, they may be considered to be conceptually distinguishable. These two notions of (in)distinguishability have quite different physical origins, the former one being related to observations while the latter has to do with the preparation of the system. 相似文献
63.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review - We present a critical review of computational models of generalization of simple grammar-like rules, such as ABA and ABB. In particular, we focus on models... 相似文献
64.
Verwey WB Abrahamse EL Ruitenberg MF Jiménez L de Kleine E 《Psychological research》2011,75(5):406-422
The present study examined whether middle-aged participants, like young adults, learn movement patterns by preparing and executing
integrated sequence representations (i.e., motor chunks) that eliminate the need for external guidance of individual movements. Twenty-four middle-aged participants (aged 55–62)
practiced two fixed key press sequences, one including three and one including six key presses in the discrete sequence production task. Their performance was compared with that of 24 young adults (aged 18–28). In the middle-aged participants motor chunks
as well as explicit sequence knowledge appeared to be less developed than in the young adults. This held especially with respect
to the unstructured 6-key sequences in which most middle-aged did not develop independence of the key-specific stimuli and
learning seems to have been based on associative learning. These results are in line with the notion that sequence learning
involves several mechanisms and that aging affects the relative contribution of these mechanisms. 相似文献
65.
Studies in several languages have shown that subject-relative clauses are easier to process than object-relative clauses. Mak, Vonk, and Schriefers (2006) have proposed the topichood hypothesis to account for the preference for subject-relative clauses. This hypothesis claims that the entity in the relative clause that is most topicworthy will be chosen as the subject. By default, the antecedent of the relative clause will be chosen as the subject of the relative clause, because it is the topic of the relative clause. However, when the noun phrase (NP) in the relative clause is also topicworthy, the preference for the antecedent to be the subject will disappear. This was confirmed in two experiments. In Experiment 1, we tested relative clauses with a personal pronoun in the relative clause. We obtained a preference for object-relative clauses, in line with the assumption that personal pronouns refer to a discourse topic and are thus topicworthy. In Experiment 2, the discourse status of the NP in the relative clause was manipulated; either it was not present in the preceding context, or it was the discourse topic. The experiment showed that when the NP in the relative clause refers to the discourse topic, the difficulty of object-relative clauses is reduced, in comparison with relative clauses with an NP that is new in the discourse, even in the absence of any explicit cue in the relative clause itself. The experiments show that discourse factors guide processing at the sentence level. 相似文献
66.
Thunnissen M Duivenvoorden H Busschbach J Hakkaart-van Roijen L van Tilburg W Verheul R Trijsburg W 《Journal of personality disorders》2008,22(5):483-495
Although several studies show symptomatic improvements in patients with personality disorders after short-term inpatient psychotherapy, reintegration remains difficult. In this study the effectiveness of a specifically designed reintegration training program is investigated. One hundred twenty-eight patients were randomized to either a reintegration training program aimed at improving general functioning and work resumption, or booster sessions. Outcome measures used were symptom level, work status, absence from and impediments at work. The results showed that compliance in the booster session group was significantly better than in the reintegration training program. The percentage of persons with a paid job increased during the booster sessions from 64 to 87%, but not during the reintegration training (76%). There were no differences in the other outcome measures. We concluded that reintegration training was not more (cost)-effective than booster sessions. Our hypothesis is that continuity of care (same therapists and program) explains the favorable results of the booster sessions. 相似文献
67.
68.
Various studies suggest that movement sequences are initially learned predominantly in effector-independent spatial coordinates
and only after extended practice in effector-dependent coordinates. The present study examined this notion for the discrete
sequence production (DSP) task by manipulating the hand used and the position of the hand relative to the body. During sequence
learning in Experiment 1, in which sequences were executed by reacting to key-specific cues, hand position appeared important
for execution with the practiced but not with the unpracticed hand. In Experiment 2 entire sequences were executed by reacting
to one cue. This produced similar results as in Experiment 1. These experiments support the notion that robustness of sequencing
skill is based on several codes, one being a representation that is both effector and position dependent. 相似文献
69.
Earlier research has established that speakers usually fixate the objects they name and that the viewing time for an object depends on the time necessary for object recognition and for the retrieval of its name. In three experiments, speakers produced pronouns and noun phrases to refer to new objects and to objects already known. Speakers looked less frequently and for shorter periods at the objects to be named when they had very recently seen or heard of these objects than when the objects were new. Looking rates were higher and viewing times longer in preparation of noun phrases than in preparation of pronouns. If it is assumed that there is a close relationship between eye gaze and visual attention, these results reveal (1) that speakers allocate less visual attention to given objects than to new ones and (2) that they allocate visual attention both less often and for shorter periods to objects they will refer to by a pronoun than to objects they will name in a full noun phrase. The experiments suggest that linguistic processing benefits, directly or indirectly, from allocation of visual attention to the referent object. 相似文献
70.