全文获取类型
收费全文 | 265篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
268篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 7篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 29篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 18篇 |
2010年 | 4篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 12篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有268条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
Declining marginal utility (DMU) describes the reduced value that additional units of money or consumer goods have in comparison
to units acquired earlier. We extend this idea to social resources and activities such as socializing time, free time, and
number of children, suggesting that most resources will show DMU as the person experiences more of them. In Study 1, participants
reported how many years of adult life they would sacrifice to have increasingly more of each resource or activity. Income
showed declining marginal utility, but other goods showed an ideal-level pattern in which they were valued less after an optimal amount. In Phase 2 of Study 1, we assessed the mix of activities people most prefer. Participants rated
the desirability of various combinations of time spent in different activities. Spending all of one’s free time in a pleasant
activity was not as desirable as spending some of the time in other desirable, but less enjoyable, activities. In Study 2,
we used a representative sample of the world to assess people’s affect balance (positive minus negative moods) on the previous
day, along with how much time they spent in activities such as socializing with family and friends. The most popular activity
was socializing with family and friends, but even here there was DMU for more hours of this behavior. We also analyzed several
forms of well-being in reference to household income. Income showed a clear DMU pattern using daily moods as the outcome,
as well as for life evaluations and satisfaction with standard of living. The results of the two studies explain why people
do not pursue happiness by spending all of their time in the most pleasant activities, such as socializing, but instead choose
a mixture of activities. A desire for balance in human activities and resources has important implications for the structure
of the workplace, leisure time, work hours, and other important domains of life. 相似文献
152.
One of the greatest strengths and liabilities of the career field is its diversity. This diversity allows for wide coverage of relevant career dynamics across the lifespan and across levels of analysis. However, this diversity also reflects fragmentation, with career scholars failing to appreciate how the insights from other thought worlds can advance their own work. Using advanced bibliometric mapping techniques, we provide a systematic review of the 3141 articles on careers published in the management literature between 1990 and 2012. In doing so, we (1) map key terms to create a systematic taxonomy of career studies within the field of management studies, (2) provide a synthetic overview of each topic cluster which extends prior reviews of more limited scope, and (3) identify the most highly influential studies on careers within each cluster. Specifically, six local clusters emerged — i.e., international careers, career management, career choice, career adaptation, individual and relational career success, and life opportunities. To classify a broad range of research opportunities for career scholars, we also create a “global” map of 16,146 career articles from across the social sciences. Specifically, six global clusters emerged — i.e., organizational, individual, education, doctorate careers, high-profile careers, and social policy. We describe and compare the clusters in the map with an emphasis on those avenues career scholars in management have yet to explore. 相似文献
153.
Kyle J.A. Small 《Teaching Theology & Religion》2014,17(4):397-399
154.
155.
Will M. Gervais Catherine L. Reed Paula M. Beall Ralph J. Roberts 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2010,72(6):1437-1443
Research confirms that the body influences perception, but little is known about the embodiment of attention. We investigated whether the implied actions of others direct spatial attention, using a lateralized covert-orienting task with nonpredictive central cues depicting static, right/left-facing bodies poised in midaction. Validity effects (decreased response times for validly compared with invalidly cued trials) indicated orienting in the direction of the implied action. In Experiment 1, we compared action (running, throwing) with nonaction (standing) cues. Only the action cues produced validity effects, suggesting that implied action directs attention. The action cues produced faster responses overall, suggesting that action cues prime motor responses. In Experiment 2, we determined whether action cues shifted attention in a specific direction rather than to a general side of space: Two cues had similar action speed and motor effort but differed in implied direction (jumping, vertical; throwing, horizontal). Validity effects were found only for the throw cues for which the implied motion direction was consistent with lateralized target locations. In Experiment 3, we compared block-like stimuli to the throwing action stimuli to examine whether lower level perceptual information could account for the attention effects alone. Validity effects were found only for the human-action stimuli. Overall, the results suggest that predictive simulations of action shift attention in action-consistent directions. 相似文献
156.
Atheists have long been distrusted, in part because they do not believe that a watchful, judging god monitors their behavior. However, in many parts of the world, secular institutions such as police, judges, and courts are also potent sources of social monitoring that encourage prosocial behavior. Reminders of such secular authority could therefore reduce believers' distrust of atheists. In our experiments, participants who watched a video about police effectiveness (Experiment 1) or were subtly primed with secular-authority concepts (Experiments 2-3) expressed less distrust of atheists than did participants who watched a control video or were not primed, respectively. We tested three distinct alternative explanations for these findings. Compared with control participants, participants primed with secular-authority concepts did not exhibit reduced general prejudice against out-groups (Experiment 1), prejudice reactions associated with functional threats that particular out-groups are perceived to pose (specifically, viewing gays with disgust; Experiment 2), or general distrust of out-groups (Experiment 3). These findings contribute to theory regarding both the psychological bases of prejudices and the psychological functions served by gods and governments. 相似文献
157.
158.
Gervais WM Shariff AF Norenzayan A 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2011,101(6):1189-1206
159.
The present study examined the effects of executive function (i.e., EF) and anger/hostility on the relationship between stress (across individual stress domains, as well as at the aggregate level) and aggression. Two independent groups of participants-a college sample and a low-income community sample-were administered a battery of self-report measures concerning the subjective experience of stress, aggressive behaviors, and feelings of state anger and hostility in the last month, along with a battery of well-validated neuropsychological tests of EF. Across both samples, the stress domains that demonstrated the strongest associations with aggression were those involving chronic strains of daily living (e.g., job, financial, health) versus interpersonal stressors (e.g., family, romantic). In the community sample, analyses also revealed a significant interaction between perceived stress (aggregated across domains) and EF in predicting aggressive behavior. Specifically, participants with relatively low EF abilities, across different EF processes, showed a stronger relationship between different domains of stress and aggression in the last month. Similar effects were demonstrated in the college sample, although the interaction was not significant. In both samples, experiences of anger and hostility in the last month mediated the relationship between perceived stress (aggregate) and aggressive behavior among those low, but not high, in EF. These findings highlight the importance of higher-order cognitive processes in regulating appropriate affective and behavioral responses across different types of individuals, particularly among those experiencing high levels of stress. 相似文献
160.