全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1670篇 |
免费 | 29篇 |
专业分类
1699篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 33篇 |
2019年 | 52篇 |
2018年 | 38篇 |
2017年 | 48篇 |
2016年 | 50篇 |
2015年 | 30篇 |
2014年 | 34篇 |
2013年 | 185篇 |
2012年 | 60篇 |
2011年 | 83篇 |
2010年 | 56篇 |
2009年 | 45篇 |
2008年 | 72篇 |
2007年 | 67篇 |
2006年 | 73篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 46篇 |
2003年 | 52篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 33篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 28篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 20篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 20篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1973年 | 20篇 |
排序方式: 共有1699条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
Meagan E. Sumter Margaret R. Gifford Jeffrey H. Tiger Hannah M. Effertz Caitlin J. Fulton 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2020,53(4):2319-2329
Functional Communication Training (FCT) involves arranging extinction for problem behavior and reinforcement for a more desirable, functionally equivalent, communicative response (FCR). Although effective under ideal arrangements, the introduction of delays to reinforcement following the FCR can result in increased problem behavior. Austin and Tiger (2015) showed that for individuals whose problem behavior was sensitive to multiple sources of reinforcement, providing access to alternative, functional reinforcers during delays mitigated this increase in problem behavior during delay fading. The current study replicated the procedures of Austin and Tiger with 2 individuals displaying multiply controlled problem behavior. Providing alternative functional reinforcers reduced problem behavior during 10-min delays for both participants without requiring delay fading. 相似文献
53.
Alex McConville Margaret Wetherell Tim McCreanor Belinda Borell Helen Moewaka Barnes 《Political psychology》2020,41(1):129-144
This article explores affect, colonial privilege, and the cultural politics of national commemoration in Aotearoa New Zealand. Based on focus-group interviews around two major national days, we examine means through which feelings and emotions are deployed in ways that enable the reproduction of social advantage. Situating affect within patterns of relationship, four interrelated affective-discursive practices are explored. In relation to Waitangi Day, agents tend to work under the rubric of anger and confusion. For Anzac Day, being grateful and moved shapes the interaction, although participants often indicate preferences towards “having a day off.” Given the colonial context in which these practices circulate, analysis observes the associated freedom and ease by which affective-discursive privilege is (re)produced. Often incongruent and rarely challenged, privilege allows associated actors to do what they want, when they want, however they want. This affective climate authorizes the ongoing reproduction of, and justification for, membership to a higher-status ethnic group of which unearned opportunities and entitlements remain its everyday, expected currency. 相似文献
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
Brett D. Wilkinson Michelle Saltis John A. Dewell 《The Journal of Humanistic Counseling》2020,59(1):54-70
The authors examine how the cognitive complexity domains of differentiation and integration are uniquely cultivated by constructivist and phenomenological teaching practices, respectively. Implications are explored in terms of reflective practices and proposed phenomenological activities that support concept deconstruction and empathy development as a means to grow integrative complexity. 相似文献
59.
Superconditioning is said to occur when learning an association between a conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US) is facilitated by pairing the CS with the US in the presence of a previously established conditioned inhibitor. Previous demonstrations of superconditioning have been criticized because their control conditions have allowed alternative interpretations. Using a within–subjects autoshaping procedure, the present study unambiguously demonstrated superconditioning. The results support the view that superconditioning is the symmetric opposite of blocking. 相似文献
60.
In thecandle problem (Duncker, 1945), subjects must attach a candle to a vertical surface, using only a box of tacks and a book of matches. Subjects
exhibitfunctional fixedness by failing, or being slow, to make use of one object (the tack box) as a support, rather than as a container, in their solutions.
This failure to produce alternate functions is measured against improved performance when the tack box is presented empty
rather than full of tacks (i.e., not preutilized as a container). Using an analogous task, we show that functional fixedness
can be demonstrated in older children (6- and 7-year-olds); they are significantly slower to use a box as a support when its
containment function has been demonstrated than when it has not. However, younger children (5-year-olds) are immune to this
effect, showing no advantage when the standard function is not demonstrated. Moreover, their performance under conditions
of preutilization is better than that of both older groups. These results are interpreted in terms of children’s developing
intuitions about function and the effects of past experience on problem solving. 相似文献