首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   164768篇
  免费   7564篇
  国内免费   155篇
  2021年   1480篇
  2020年   2757篇
  2019年   3423篇
  2018年   3397篇
  2017年   3818篇
  2016年   4536篇
  2015年   3897篇
  2014年   4780篇
  2013年   23368篇
  2012年   4265篇
  2011年   3277篇
  2010年   3753篇
  2009年   4684篇
  2008年   3543篇
  2007年   3102篇
  2006年   3780篇
  2005年   3770篇
  2004年   3287篇
  2003年   2933篇
  2002年   2711篇
  2001年   2870篇
  2000年   2773篇
  1999年   2879篇
  1998年   2798篇
  1997年   2622篇
  1996年   2555篇
  1995年   2388篇
  1994年   2360篇
  1993年   2303篇
  1992年   2367篇
  1991年   2253篇
  1990年   2090篇
  1989年   2027篇
  1988年   2004篇
  1987年   2025篇
  1986年   2011篇
  1985年   2228篇
  1984年   2380篇
  1983年   2204篇
  1982年   2330篇
  1981年   2299篇
  1980年   2154篇
  1979年   2049篇
  1978年   2107篇
  1977年   2085篇
  1976年   1866篇
  1975年   1885篇
  1974年   1925篇
  1973年   1798篇
  1972年   1409篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
192.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
193.
194.
195.
196.
197.
198.
Several studies have illuminated how processing manual action verbs (MaVs) affects the programming or execution of concurrent hand movements. Here, to circumvent key confounds in extant designs, we conducted the first assessment of motor–language integration during handwriting—a task in which linguistic and motoric processes are co‐substantiated. Participants copied MaVs, non‐manual action verbs, and non‐action verbs as we collected measures of motor programming and motor execution. Programming latencies were similar across conditions, but execution was faster for MaVs than for the other categories, regardless of whether word meanings were accessed implicitly or explicitly. In line with the Hand‐Action‐Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model, such findings suggest that effector‐congruent verbs can prime manual movements even during highly automatized tasks in which motoric and verbal processes are naturally intertwined. Our paradigm opens new avenues for fine‐grained explorations of embodied language processes.  相似文献   
199.
200.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号