全文获取类型
收费全文 | 130篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
130篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 4篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 8篇 |
2012年 | 3篇 |
2011年 | 3篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 3篇 |
2007年 | 4篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 2篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 2篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有130条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Language access and theory of mind reasoning: evidence from deaf children in bilingual and oralist environments 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Meristo M Falkman KW Hjelmquist E Tedoldi M Surian L Siegal M 《Developmental psychology》2007,43(5):1156-1169
This investigation examined whether access to sign language as a medium for instruction influences theory of mind (ToM) reasoning in deaf children with similar home language environments. Experiment 1 involved 97 deaf Italian children ages 4-12 years: 56 were from deaf families and had LIS (Italian Sign Language) as their native language, and 41 had acquired LIS as late signers following contact with signers outside their hearing families. Children receiving bimodal/bilingual instruction in LIS together with Sign-Supported and spoken Italian significantly outperformed children in oralist schools in which communication was in Italian and often relied on lipreading. Experiment 2 involved 61 deaf children in Estonia and Sweden ages 6-16 years. On a wide variety of ToM tasks, bilingually instructed native signers in Estonian Sign Language and spoken Estonian succeeded at a level similar to age-matched hearing children. They outperformed bilingually instructed late signers and native signers attending oralist schools. Particularly for native signers, access to sign language in a bilingual environment may facilitate conversational exchanges that promote the expression of ToM by enabling children to monitor others' mental states effectively. 相似文献
82.
Studia Logica - Two different kinds of multiple-conclusion consequence relations taken from Shoesmith and Smiley (Multiple-conclusion logic, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1978) and Galatos... 相似文献
83.
The present study supplemented the limited 1999 psychometric data by Lewis, et al. on the Czech translation of the Satisfaction With Life Scale by examining the temporal stability of the measure over a 1-wk. period for 64 Czech undergraduate university students. Analysis indicated stability across the two administrations was very high (r=.90), and there was no significant change between Time 1 (M=23.7, SD=5.1) and Time 2 (M=24.1, SD=5.5). Short-term test-retest reliability of this Czech translation is supported. 相似文献
84.
Statistics of Intuitionistic versus Classical Logics 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:1
For the given logical calculus we investigate the proportion of the number of true formulas of a certain length n to the number of all formulas of such length. We are especially interested in asymptotic behavior of this fraction when n tends to infinity. If the limit exists it is represented by a real number between 0 and 1 which we may call the density of truth for the investigated logic. In this paper we apply this approach to the intuitionistic logic of one variable with implication and negation. The result is obtained by reducing the problem to the same one of Dummett's intermediate linear logic of one variable (see [2]). Actually, this paper shows the exact density of intuitionistic logic and demonstrates that it covers a substantial part (more than 93%) of classical prepositional calculus. Despite using strictly mathematical means to solve all discussed problems, this paper in fact, may have a philosophical impact on understanding how much the phenomenon of truth is sporadic or frequent in random mathematics sentences. 相似文献
85.
Lukasz D. Kaczmarek Todd B. Kashdan Evan M. Kleiman Blazej Baczkowski Jolanta Enko Adrianna Siebers Agata Szäefer Marek Król Barbara Baran 《Personality and individual differences》2013
Despite a variety of interventions to increase well-being, little is known about who is interested in and initiates exercises on their own. We explored individual differences that predict who is most likely to participate in a voluntary gratitude intervention. College students (n = 226) completed measures of curiosity, depressive symptoms, life satisfaction, and intentions to change their lifestyle. Afterwards, participants received a personalized invitation to take part in a web-based intervention to enhance their well-being (anonymous and strictly voluntary). Results suggested that 11.5% of participants started the gratitude intervention. Individuals endorsing strong intentions to change their lifestyle (+1 SD above mean) were 2.2 times more likely than their peers to start the gratitude intervention. People with greater trait curiosity endorsed greater intentions to start this intervention; people with greater depressive symptoms endorsed weaker intentions. Both curiosity and depressive symptoms indirectly influenced initiation of the gratitude intervention via intentions. These findings provide support for particular paths that lead to the initial behavioral effort towards healthy change. We discuss the implications for attempting to increase and sustain people’s well-being. 相似文献
86.
Czarkowski M 《Science and engineering ethics》2010,16(1):99-110
Biological research with legitimate scientific purpose that may be misused to pose a biological threat to public health and/or
national security is termed dual use. In Poland there are adequate conditions for conducting experiments that could be qualified
as dual use research, and therefore, a risk of attack on Poland or other countries exists. Optimal solutions for limiting
such threats are required, and the national system of biosecurity should enable early, reliable, and complete identification
of this type of research. Scientists should have a fundamental role in this process, their duty being to immediately, upon
identification, report research with dual use potential. An important entity in the identification system of dual use research
should also be the Central Register of Biological and Biomedical Research, which gathers information about all biological
and biomedical research being conducted in a given country. Publishers, editors, and review committees of journals and other
scientific publications should be involved in evaluating results of clinical trials. The National Council of Biosecurity should
be the governmental institution responsible for developing a system of dual use research threat prevention. Its role would
be to develop codes of conduct, form counsel of expertise, and monitor the problem at national level, while the Dual Use Research
Committee would be responsible for individual cases. In Poland, current actions aiming to provide biological safety were based
on developing and passing an act about genetically modified organisms (GMO’s) and creating a GMO Committee. Considering experiences
of other nations, one should view these actions as fragmentary, and thus insufficient protection against dual use research
threats. 相似文献
87.
Kristopher J. Gauthier Andrew N. Christopher Mark I. Walter Ronney Mourad Pam Marek 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2006,7(2):139-154
Previous research has found a consistent, negative relationship between holding religious doubts and mental well-being, and
a small positive relationship between religiosity and mental well-being. To assess the interrelationship between religious
doubt, religiosity, and need for cognition on life satisfaction, a survey was administered to an almost exclusively Christian
sample of 192 Americans drawn from undergraduates and alumni of a small mid-western college, undergraduates from a small south-eastern
college, and several churches from the metro-Detroit area. Zero-order correlations revealed relationships between religiosity
and life satisfaction, as well as religious doubt and life satisfaction. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses revealed
that the three-way interaction of religiosity, religious doubt, and the need for cognition was predictive of life satisfaction.
Significant two-way interactions also emerged for both gender and religiosity, and gender and religious doubt as predictors
of life satisfaction. Based upon these findings, counseling applications are discussed, and the importance of probing for
interactions in research on religious influences on well-being is espoused.
Portions of this research were presented at the 2004 Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Honolulu,
HI. 相似文献
88.
The confusion/non-consequential thinking explanation proposed by Newstead, Girotto, and Legrenzi (1995) for poor performance on Wason's THOG problem (a hypothetico-deductive reasoning task) was examined in three experiments with 300 participants. In general, as the cognitive complexity of the problem and the possibility of non-consequential thinking were reduced, correct performance increased. Significant but weak facilitation (33-40% correct) was found in Experiment 1 for THOG classification instructions that did not include the indeterminate response option. Substantial facilitation (up to 75% correct) was obtained in Experiment 2 with O'Brien et al.'s (1990) one-other-THOG classification instruction. In Experiment 3, a revised version of O'Brien et al.'s pre-test problem format also led to substantial facilitation, even with the use of the standard three-choice THOG classification instruction. These findings are discussed in terms of Newstead et al.'s theoretical proposal and possible attentional factors. 相似文献
89.
The aim of the present study was to investigate the role of executive functions (EF) in theory-of-mind (ToM) performance in deaf children and adolescents. Four groups of deaf children aged 7–16 years, with different language backgrounds at home and at school, that is, bilingually instructed native signers, oralist-instructed native signers, and two groups of bilingually instructed late signers from Sweden and Estonia, respectively, were given eight ToM and four EF measures. The bilingually instructed native signers performed at a significantly higher level on the ToM measures than the other groups of deaf children. On the EF measures, there were no significant differences found between any of the groups, with one exception—the Swedish bilingual late signers had a significantly shorter average reaction time on the go-no-go inhibition task than the oralist native signers and the Estonian bilingual late signers. However, the Swedish children's better EF performance was not mirrored in better performance on ToM tasks. Our results indicate that despite all deaf children's good general cognitive abilities, there were still differences in their performance on ToM tasks that need to be explained in other terms. Thus, whatever the cause of late signers' difficulties with ToM, poor EF-skills seem to be of minor importance. 相似文献
90.
Michal Bilewicz Roland Imhoff Marek Drogosz 《European journal of social psychology》2011,41(2):201-209
Studies on dehumanization demonstrated that denying certain human characteristics might serve as a strategy for moral disengagement. Meat consumption—especially in the times of cruel animal farming—is related to the exclusion of animals from the human scope of justice. In the present research, it was hypothesized that the conception of human uniqueness (denying animals certain psychological characteristics) might be a strategy of meat‐eaters' moral disengagement. Three studies compared the extent to which vegetarians and omnivores attribute psychological characteristics to humans versus animals. In Study 1, vegetarian participants ascribed more secondary (uniquely human) emotions to animals than did the omnivores; however, there were no differences in primary (animalistic) emotions. Study 2 showed that omnivores distinguish human characteristics from animalistic ones more sharply than vegetarians do, while both groups do not differ in distinguishing human characteristics from mechanistic ones. Study 3 confirmed the results by showing that omnivores ascribed less secondary emotions to traditionally edible animals than to the non‐edible species, while vegetarians did not differentiate these animals. These results support the claim that the lay conceptions of ‘human uniqueness’ are strategies of moral disengagement. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献