全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4498篇 |
免费 | 55篇 |
专业分类
4553篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 38篇 |
2023年 | 44篇 |
2022年 | 43篇 |
2021年 | 81篇 |
2020年 | 91篇 |
2019年 | 129篇 |
2018年 | 176篇 |
2017年 | 163篇 |
2016年 | 194篇 |
2015年 | 131篇 |
2014年 | 155篇 |
2013年 | 518篇 |
2012年 | 237篇 |
2011年 | 266篇 |
2010年 | 149篇 |
2009年 | 153篇 |
2008年 | 220篇 |
2007年 | 175篇 |
2006年 | 152篇 |
2005年 | 121篇 |
2004年 | 139篇 |
2003年 | 141篇 |
2002年 | 110篇 |
2001年 | 84篇 |
2000年 | 89篇 |
1999年 | 62篇 |
1998年 | 45篇 |
1997年 | 45篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 39篇 |
1991年 | 28篇 |
1990年 | 50篇 |
1989年 | 47篇 |
1988年 | 41篇 |
1987年 | 26篇 |
1986年 | 34篇 |
1985年 | 33篇 |
1984年 | 29篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有4553条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
851.
The term “evolution” is often used in the administrative sciences to designate dynamic processes of change in general. In
biology, evolution has a very specific meaning, namely the application of a generative variation-selective retention scheme
to change. Applying this to the administrative sciences is more exacting: describing what the variation consists of, how replication
of the generated variation occurs and delineating the population from which the selection is made are far from easy. While
it is the intention of this special issue as a whole to provide some empirical examples of evolutionary change in the public
and private sectors, this introduction will lay out the main lines of thought in what is often referred to as universal Darwinism
and what this means in the administrative sciences. Key concepts here are generative variation, selective retention, and selective
institutional environment. According to the authors, the Darwinian scheme of evolutionary theory focuses on how new ideas
or concepts arise, how they propagate and influence wider actor thinking and on how the institutional environment in which
they operate affects their differential survival. This evolutionary process is an interplay between actors and replicators
(sounds, images, and in this context mostly words) in which it cannot be said in advance which one is fooling the other.
Martin De Jong is associate professor of public management.
Haiko Van der Voort is a research associate who lectures and publishes on a wide range of issues including process management
and system innovations. 相似文献
852.
853.
This paper attempts to systematically characterize critical reactions in argumentative discourse, such as objections, critical
questions, rebuttals, refutations, counterarguments, and fallacy charges, in order to contribute to the dialogical approach
to argumentation. We shall make use of four parameters to characterize distinct types of critical reaction. First, a critical
reaction has a focus, for example on the standpoint, or on another part of an argument. Second, critical reactions appeal to some kind of norm, argumentative or other. Third, they each have a particular illocutionary force, which may include that of giving strategic advice to the other. Fourth, a critical reaction occurs at a particular level of dialogue (the ground level or some meta-level). The concepts here developed shall be applied to discussions of critical
reactions by Aristotle and by some contemporary authors. 相似文献
854.
Pieter T.M. Desmet David De Cremer Eric van Dijk 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2011,114(2):75-86
Trust is vital yet vulnerable in economic exchange relations. In these relations, a widely used strategy in response to distributive harm consists of having the transgressor pay a financial compensation to the victim. This research examines whether financial compensations can increase trust towards a transgressor, and whether the size of the compensation is relevant to this process. We hypothesized and found that whether larger compensations will elicit more trust, depends on how clear the perpetrator’s intention to transgress was. Experiment 1 revealed that trust perceptions increased more by a slight overcompensation of the inflicted harm as compared to an exact or a partial compensation, but not if the transgressor’s bad intentions became clear through the use of deception in the violation. In Experiments 2 and 3, we replicated these findings and further showed that it is not the use of deception per se, but rather the attribution of bad intent that moderates the effect of compensation size. Experiment 4, using a trust game paradigm revealed that this effect not only occurs for small overcompensations, but also for larger overcompensations. 相似文献
855.
Statistical properties in the visual environment can be used to improve performance on visual working memory (VWM) tasks. The current study examined the ability to incidentally learn that a change is more likely to occur to a particular feature dimension (shape, color, or location) and use this information to improve change detection performance for that dimension (the change probability effect). Participants completed a change detection task in which one change type was more probable than others. Change probability effects were found for color and shape changes, but not location changes, and intentional strategies did not improve the effect. Furthermore, the change probability effect developed and adapted to new probability information quickly. Finally, in some conditions, an improvement in change detection performance for a probable change led to an impairment in change detection for improbable changes. 相似文献
856.
Stigma is a common phenomenon worldwide and infectious diseases like HIV/AIDS and leprosy are often associated with high levels of stigma. Several studies have been conducted concerning the effects of stigma and the impact on social participation, but comparative studies are rare. The objective of this study was to identify differences and similarities between HIV/AIDS and leprosy-related stigma. From April till July 2009, 190 questionnaire-based interviews were conducted to assess the levels of internalized stigma (Internalized Stigma of Mental Illness scale), perceived stigma (Explanatory Model Interview Catalogue stigma scale) and social participation (Participation scale) in a cross-sectional sample of people affected by leprosy (PL) and people living with HIV/AIDS (PLHA). Respondents were selected from several hospitals, charity projects and during home visits in Vellore district, Tamil Nadu. Our results showed that both PLHA (n?=?95) and leprosy-affected respondents (n?=?95) faced a substantial burden of internalized and perceived stigma, with the former reporting a significantly higher level of stigma. As a result, PLHA faced more frequent and also more severe participation restrictions than PL. Especially, restrictions in work-related areas were reported by the majority of the respondents. In conclusion, PLHA faced a significantly higher level of stigma and participation restriction than PL. However, the latter also reported a substantial burden of stigma and participation restrictions. The study suggests that it may be possible to develop joint interventions based on the commonalities found. More research is needed to define these more precisely and to test the effectiveness of such joint interventions in reducing stigma and improving social participation. 相似文献
857.
We investigated whether mindfulness training (MT) influences information processing in a working memory task with complex
visual stimuli. Participants were tested before (T1) and after (T2) participation in an intensive one-month MT retreat, and
their performance was compared with that of an age- and education-matched control group. Accuracy did not differ across groups
at either time point. Response times were faster and significantly less variable in the MT versus the control group at T2.
Since these results could be due to changes in mnemonic processes, speed–accuracy trade-off, or nondecisional factors (e.g.,
motor execution), we used a mathematical modeling approach to disentangle these factors. The EZ-diffusion model (Wagenmakers,
van der Maas, & Grasman, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14:(1), 3–22, 2007) suggested that MT leads to improved information quality and reduced response conservativeness, with no changes in nondecisional
factors. The noisy exemplar model further suggested that the increase in information quality reflected a decrease in encoding
noise and not an increase in forgetting. Thus, mathematical modeling may help clarify the mechanisms by which MT produces
salutary effects on performance. 相似文献
858.
Recently, several authors claimed that the curvilinear shape of rating-based source memory receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) refutes threshold models. However, rating-based ROCs are not diagnostic to disprove threshold models. Furthermore, source memory ROC-analyses ignore influences of other processes like old-new-detection and old-new-response-tendencies, so direct estimation of parameters is preferable. Five source monitoring experiments with different response bias manipulations and materials were conducted. We fitted the Two High Threshold Multinomial Model of Source Monitoring by Bayen, Murnane, and Erdfelder (1996) and the Multivariate Signal Detection Model for Selection DeCarlo (2003). We also included rating-based ROCs. The results suggest that both models are at least equally valid as measurement tools which capture bias processes in the corresponding parameters, and they can perhaps be integrated theoretically. 相似文献
859.
This special issue of the journal‘Studies in Logic’ results from an international workshopon‘TheHistory of Logic in China’held in Amsterdam,The Netherlands,on November 24-26, 2010 (http://www.sciencehistory.asia/history-logic-china).This event brought together, 相似文献
860.
Ruotolo F van der Ham IJ Iachini T Postma A 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2011,64(6):1138-1156
We report two experiments on the relationship between allocentric/egocentric frames of reference and categorical/coordinate spatial relations. Jager and Postma (2003) suggest two theoretical possibilities about their relationship: categorical judgements are better when combined with an allocentric reference frame and coordinate judgements with an egocentric reference frame (interaction hypothesis); allocentric/egocentric and categorical/coordinate form independent dimensions (independence hypothesis). Participants saw stimuli comprising two vertical bars (targets), one above and the other below a horizontal bar. They had to judge whether the targets appeared on the same side (categorical) or at the same distance (coordinate) with respect either to their body-midline (egocentric) or to the centre of the horizontal bar (allocentric). The results from Experiment 1 showed a facilitation in the allocentric and categorical conditions. In line with the independence hypothesis, no interaction effect emerged. To see whether the results were affected by the visual salience of the stimuli, in Experiment 2 the luminance of the horizontal bar was reduced. As a consequence, a significant interaction effect emerged indicating that categorical judgements were more accurate than coordinate ones, and especially so in the allocentric condition. Furthermore, egocentric judgements were as accurate as allocentric ones with a specific improvement when combined with coordinate spatial relations. The data from Experiment 2 showed that the visual salience of stimuli affected the relationship between allocentric/egocentric and categorical/coordinate dimensions. This suggests that the emergence of a selective interaction between the two dimensions may be modulated by the characteristics of the task. 相似文献